Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
Developmental case study on attachment
Strength and weakness of research studies of attachment
Application of attachment theory
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
Recommended: Developmental case study on attachment
Adam Bengtson Paper 1 Psych 301 9/25/14 Schwartz (1983) investigated how length of day-care attendance correlated to attachment behavior in eighteen-month-old infants. Schwartz’s study set out to determine the differences in attachment behavior of eighteen-month-old infants in relation to their length of day-care attendance or lack of attendance. This study specifically focuses on eighteen-month-old infants from middle-class families where all of the infants had been placed into full time day-care before the age of 9 months, part time day-care or if they attended no day-care at all. In conducting this study Schwartz observed fifty 18-month-old infants and their mothers in order to see the effects of daily separation the between the mothers …show more content…
and their children. These infants and their mothers were observed twice. Once they were observed in their normal home environment and then once again in the laboratory setting. Based on the recorded results of these sessions they were then separated into one of three categories. These categories were dependent on if the infant attended a full time day-care, part time day-care or if they attended no day-care at all. During these two sessions Schwartz recorded the strength and persistence of behaviors that included contact seeking and maintenance, proximity avoidance, and contact resistance. In the home environment Schwartz recorded that there were no major differences of behavior between the infants who attended full time day-care, part time day-care, or attended no day-care. However, it is noted that the mothers with infants that attended full time or part time day-care showed a more positive and a more affectionate behavior toward their child in comparison to those mothers that did not have their child in day-care. In the other setting, the laboratory, infants who attended a full time day-care showed an increase in avoidance behavior in comparison to infants that attended part time day-care or no day-care. The attachment behavior between the infants and their mother does appear to be influenced by the separation and amount of time and the infant spends at day-care. Schuetze, Lewis, and DiMartino (1999) studied the relation between time spent in day-care and exploratory behaviors in 9-month-old infants. For this experiment Scheutze, Lewis, and DiMartino studied 32 healthy full-term nine-month-old infants; five of these infants had previously not been enrolled in a daycare. 18 of subjects were boys and the other 14 girls. These subjects were found through word-of-mouth among three local daycare centers. Testing for this study was conducted in two areas. For those enrolled in day-care, testing was done in a quite, familiar area. For the infants in home-based care the research was done in a laboratory setting. At all times of the testing a known caregiver was present with the infant. Once the infant had time to familiarize with their testing environment, different measures of exploratory behaviors were then attained. These test included how the infant chose to free play with toys having no contact with the caregiver, other than encouraging statements. Other test also included simple problem-solving measures. Scheutze, Lewis, and DiMartino found that the amount of time an infant spends in day-care leads to more frequent and varied exploratory behaviors and a higher success of problem-solving abilities. The amount of time spent in day-care lead to the possibility of increasing motivation and development of exploratory strategies among other developments, where as the infants not enrolled in day-care showed a decline in these behaviors. These results were all dependent on the quality of care provided within the day-care. Belsky, Burchinalm, Clarke-Stewart, McCartney, Owen, and Vandell (2007) are researching if there are any long-term effects of early child-care. For this study Belsky et al. selected 1,364 families with 1-month-old infants to become the participants. Of these participants some withdrew, most had some missing data, and only 293 of the children had complete data recorded for all predictors and outcomes in any analysis. In this study the children’s experiences in child care before entry into school, for school and after-school experiences, and the child’s outcomes for social and cognitive behaviors were described. Observational assessments were conducted in specific periods throughout the child’s life in order to show the effects of early child-care. Observing the child-care type and quality. The results of this study infer that the stronger and more consistent predictor of the children’s development was parenting. As for quality of care, the higher the quality the more likely the child was to have higher vocabulary scores. The study has also shown that with a higher exposure to central care, the child is likely to have more teacher-reported externalizing problems. Egeland and Hiester (1995) researched the long-term consequences of infant day-care and mother-infant attachment. In their study, Egeland and Hiester attempted to determine if the effects of day-care over a long period of time changed dependent on the security of attachment in infancy. Another point was if the children’s adaptation to home and day-care could be predicted on the level of attachment the child had received during infancy. This study tried to show the effects of Day-care compared to home-care and the secure of insecure levels of the children. The mothers and children in this study were all considered to be at a high risk of poverty. The infants and mothers were observed over an extended period of time.
Infants were observed at 12 months old with procedures that consisted of a series of episodes. These episodes included attachment and exploration behaviors in circumstances that became gradually stressful. The infants were also observed based on their behaviors once they were reunited with their mother after separation for a short period of time. At 42 months the mothers respect for autonomy, hostility, and the child’s avoidance of the mother. They were then observed in social withdrawal, self-esteem, self-control, aggression, emotional health, and externalizing upon entering kindergarten. Results of this study show that the children who were securely attached to their mother had a higher chance to be negatively affected by early out-of-home care where as for the insecurely attached children it proved to be more beneficial. The insecure children that entered day care proved to have a higher level of proactivity in comparison to home-cared. As for the secure children who attended day-care, they came to show more aggression and became more …show more content…
avoidant. Field (1991) shows the correlation of quality infant day-care and grade school behavior and performance in two studies.
The first study consisted of 28 children averaging seven years of age. All of these children had full-time infant day-care before the age of 2 with an average of 6.7 months. The children also remained in the same nursery school before starting grade school. Packets were sent to the mothers to rate their own introverted/extroverted behavior and included a number of background questions on the child such as age, participation in gifted programs, and awards the child has received. The children were also sent a questionnaire packet included a self-concept scale and magic markers, with which the children were directed to draw a self-portrait. These portraits were judged by use of colors, placing of body parts, size of drawing, and facial expresions. Results of this study show that the number of friends the children had was positively related to the time spent in quality day-care. Based off the children’s drawings the study also notes that the more time a child spends in quality day-care leads being less despressed and having a higher self-esteem. The children who spent more time in quality day-care have also shown better socially and emotionally behaviors at the early grade school
periods. Volling and Feagans (1995) investigated children’s social competence in relation to day-care. In this investigation Volling and Feagans examined the socioeconomic status, family relations, temperament, and quality/quantity of day-care. These were examined as predictors of the child’s social interactions. 36 children were included in this study and attended one of three day-cares. The day-cares differed such as non-profit and two for-profit daycares in different environments. Questionnaires were given to the mothers that evaluated their family environment, social status, day experiences, and temperament. The children were observed on their social behavior in the classrooms when they were around 2 ½ years old. Additional observations were also made, but were not of central importance. In order to measure the children’s social competence, the children were watched in two 10-minute observations of indoor free play on two separate occasions in which an observational system was developed to evaluate the social behavior of the children. Results of this study concluded that children who attended day-care more hours per week were involved in more social play and had friendlier peer interactions. If the child-to-caregiver ratio became too large, it was noticed that there was more nonsocial play among the children. With a smaller child-to-caregiver ratio the children displayed more positive interactions. Day-care quality shows to play a main part in children’s play and interactions among each other and caregivers. Field, Masi, Goldstein, Perry, Parl examined the possible ties that day-care facilities have with children’s preschool social behaviors. Field et al. studied seventy-one preschool-aged children. Entering day-care at varying times and receiving varied amounts of care, these children were compared on their behavior when reunited with their parent, the teacher and parent ratings of their behavior, and also the interactions they displayed on during playground play. The findings from this study further shows that with more hours and months of day-care, children are more likely to oblige in cooperative play, and have more positive interactions among peers. A child receiving continuous care in quality day-care does not appear to negatively affect attachment behavior, but rather aid in the preschool social behavior. Burchinal, Bryant, Lee, and Ramey examined children put into day care early, infant-mother attachment, and the maternal responsiveness in the infant’s first year.
Chapter seven of “Making the “Terrible” Twos Terrific” by John Rosemond is about sending your children to daycare. Rosemond believes that if possible one of the parent stays at home for the first three years of life. “The first three years of life constitute the single most critical, precedent-setting developmental period” (Rosemond 207). He also talks about putting your child in a three day or half day daycare. From birth to age two children rely completely on their parents. At age three is the transition time when children learn they are not the center of the universe and this should be taught by the parents.
From birth to about six months old, an infant doesnÂ’t seem to mind staying with an unfamiliar person (Brazelton, 1992), although the infant is able to distinguish his mother from other people (Slater, et al, 1998). As the infant gets a little older, at about eight to ten months, he/she begins to cry when his caregiver is not his mother or father; and again between eighteen and twenty-four months, when the infant finds out he/she has some control over what happens (Schuster, 1980). Separation anxiety could, and often does, make parents feel guilty for leaving their child and might make them wonder if they are causing their child undue stress.
The attachment theory, presented by Mary Ainsworth in 1969 and emerged by John Bowlby suggests that the human infant has a need for a relationship with an adult caregiver, and without a subsequent, development can be negatively impacted (Hammonds 2012). Ainsworth proposes that the type of relationship and “attachment” an infant has with the caregiver, can impact the social development of the infant. As stated by Hammonds (2012), attachment between a mother and a child can have a great impact on the child 's future mental
An infant’s initial contact with the world and their exploration of life is directly through the parent/ primary caregiver. As the child grows, learns, and develops, a certain attachment relationship forms between them and the principle adult present in this process. Moreover, this attachment holds huge implications concerning the child’s future relationships and social successes. Children trust that their parental figure will be there; as a result, children whom form proper attachments internalize an image of their world as stable, safe, and secure. These children will grow independent while at the same time maintaining a connection with their caregivers. (Day, 2006). However, when a child f...
The “strange situation” is a laboratory assessment used to measure infants’ attachment to their mothers through observation, by providing a series’ of experiences of introductions to a stranger, separations from mother and then the reunion of the two. The “strange situation” measures the infants’ attachment in being securely to insecurely attached with the mother. The scene of the experiment is set up in a small room with mother, child and stranger, they will then follow a prescribed order of the series listed above. At first it’s only mother and child, and then the stranger joins the room. The mother tends to the child briefly and then leaves the room, leaving the child with the stranger. The child then expresses distress to a certain extent – depending on the degree of secure attachment to the mother – upon the mother’s departure and cries in calling the mother back. The mother returns and stranger leaves. The next step is then that the mother leaves the child completely alone, and when child then cries in a call for them to come back, the stranger will return instead of the mother, this is an important part of the experiment in order to determine the child’s attachment. It gets determined whether or not the child will return to a calm state of mind with the calming of the stranger. The mother will finally return to the child again and stranger will leave as the experiment and observation
John Bowlby’s attachment theory established that an infant’s earliest relationship with their primary caregiver or mother shaped their later development and characterized their human life, “from the cradle to the grave” (Bowlby, 1979, p. 129). The attachment style that an infant develops with their parent later reflects on their self-esteem, well-being and the romantic relationships that they form. Bowlby’s attachment theory had extensive research done by Mary Ainsworth, who studied the mother-infant interactions specifically regarding the theme of an infant’s exploration of their surrounding and the separation from their mother in an experiment called the strange situation. Ainsworth defined the four attachment styles: secure, insecure/resistant, insecure/avoidant and disorganized/disoriented, later leading to research studies done to observe this behavior and how it affects a child in their adolescence and adulthood.
Greenberg, M. T. (1990) Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press.
When selecting childcare for your child the foundation of early learning has offered some tips that they believe will be helpful in the process. The foundation says that parents have always known that good early experience was important for their child. Now scientist and researchers are confirming how critical these first years of life are to your child’s healthy development (Selecting child care, 2002). Because the brain matures in the world rather than in the womb, the brain growth and development of infants and young children is deeply affected by their earliest experiences. In a childcare environment the relationship your child has with the caregiver will also affect how that child feels about himself and the world around him and as he grows up (Selecting child care, 2002). I ...
Infant attachment is the first relationship a child experiences and is crucial to the child’s survival (BOOK). A mother’s response to her child will yield either a secure bond or insecurity with the infant. Parents who respond “more sensitively and responsively to the child’s distress” establish a secure bond faster than “parents of insecure children”. (Attachment and Emotion, page 475) The quality of the attachment has “profound implications for the child’s feelings of security and capacity to form trusting relationships” (Book). Simply stated, a positive early attachment will likely yield positive physical, socio-emotional, and cognitive development for the child. (BOOK)
It is common knowledge that a parent is considered the most efficient caregiver for their children. It’s also known that with daily responsibilities of caring for a child financially, parents partake in full-time and/or part-time employment. While needing to do so, many children attend daycare/preschool facilities. Granted, it is the parent’s responsibility to cautiously select where they decide to take their children. This is because parents know that while they are away for numerous hours of the day, their children are in the hands of another care provider and that their care would have an enormous impact on their children. At a young age, a child’s social and cognitive skills are continuing to take shape and the amount of time spent in these facilities has a resilient impact on a child’s development. With proper and superior care no matter the time spent, such positive effects on a child’s development should endure in a child’s cognitive and social development. In other words, there is a great benefit of childcare/daycare attendance on a child’s development.
Each and every daycare has their own menu for the children’s breakfast and lunch. Through my observations, I have noticed that the “Don’t Worry Childcare” has a variety of food selections throughout the month. For example, the children could either choose from fruit loops cereal, or frosted flakes for breakfast. The children’s snacks eaten varies every day as well. For instance, the children ate milk and cookies one day, and cheese crackers another day. An example of lunch would be, milk, chicken fingers, French fries, green beans, and mixed fruit one day, and spaghetti, corn, peaches, and bread another day. I observed the children eat ham, mashed potatoes, green beans, and peaches one day for lunch. All those food options seemed very delicious.
In this way, the primary caregiver acts as the main liaison between the child and his or her parents, and the two parties work as a team to provide the child with the best quality care while promoting the development of secure attachment. Although a daycare setting might not be the most ideal option in comparison to constant time spent with a parent, this intervention shows that it is still possible for working parents to achieve secure attachment with their child, even when Parents who choose this option as a method for creating and maintaining secure attachment must be willing to set aside a block of free time in the day dedicated to bonding and interacting with their child in a one-on-one setting. This way, the child is able to separate time spent with the childcare provider from time spent with the parent, and is able to utilize both relationships in developing a strong secure
The idea of taking children to day-care is unacceptable by a good number of people. They probably think this way, because daycare involves putting someone else in the ‘parenting’ picture; as far as spending time with, and caring for the child is concerned. However, this is not exactly negative. This is because day-care provides care for little children while their parents attend to other matters, such as attending school or working to earn a living. It is important for those people who discourage day care to realize that not all parents can afford to stay at home with their children all day, as much as they would like to. This is because they need to attend to other pressing matters. In the end, the child benefits from this as the living earned by the parent during this time, or even education earned during this time, will benefit the child in future.
I gave birth to a healthy, beautiful baby girl named Luna. Jeffrey has taken quite a liking to being a big brother. He likes to hold and rocker her and helps with feeding her. Jeffrey is still a slow-to warm child. He can communicate fairly well and is now using mostly complete sentences and seems to understand most of what I say. He occasionally makes errors of overregularization. He has taken an interest in music and singsongs even though he usually repeats a certain part a lot. I encourage this by taking him to local performances of children’s musicals. Jeffrey has learned the routines pretty well and is reasonably cooperative for his age. He is rarely aggressive to adults or other children. Jeffrey’s fantasy play has become more elaborate and sometimes includes superheroes or cartoon
Studies show that day cares have an effect on the social-emotional development of children. The time that children spend in day care has negative effects in social development. Children in their early years who spend many hours in day care have less social competence and cooperation, more problem behaviors, negative mood, aggression, and conflict.