Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
Human development theories
Naturalistic observation topics
Naturalistic Observation Research Paper
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
Recommended: Human development theories
The first few years of human development has a significant impact on the person’s overall growth and progress. Since there are well-defined developmental milestones for each particular stage of infancy, interactions between a 15 month old (mo) female infant and her thirty-four year old female caregiver were observed to relate theoretical concepts of human development to infant behaviour. Naturalistic Observation This set of naturalistic observations occurred in the living room of the infant’s home during the morning with only her female caregiver present. The infant babbled, answered “yes” and “no,” and used baby signs, which is characteristic of infants between 10 to 15mo, whenever her caregiver engaged the infant with infant-directed speech …show more content…
(IDS) throughout the observation. Her actions indicate that she was willing and able to engage in reciprocal communication and social interaction, suggesting that her language development depends on whether she wants to actively participate in such instances of intercommunication. In addition to this, the use of IDS by caregivers facilitate language development by promoting an infant’s attention to language, social interaction, and perceptual narrowing (Golinkoff et. al, 2015; Goldstein & Scwade, 2008). As the caregiver was preparing lunch, the infant spent time playing with tableware and utensils on the table of her highchair.
She spent time exploring these objects individually by chewing and sucking on them with her mouth, and then dropped each object off her highchair. Although she is still developing her fine motor skills, this action shows that she is capable of hand-eye coordination to achieve goal-directed behaviour that is typically for 12mo infants. By also engaging in oral exploration, she was able to aid her cognitive development by discovering additional sensory properties of the objects that she did not learn by only using her hands. The caregiver then picked up the objects for the infant and positioned the utensils into the small opening of the cups in front of the infant a couple times. The infant proceeded to put the utensils in her mouth again and was praised by her caregiver when she tried to place each eating utensil into the opening of the cup just like her caregiver had done. This instance of imitation demonstrates that observational learning allows infants to rapidly learn about behaviours and provides them the ability to successfully reproduce such behaviours at a later time. Imitation also …show more content…
requires attention to parental actions, awareness of their own abilities and knowledge of their own body parts so they can learn to perform independently and act out of self-motivation in the future (Jones, 2007). Her actions also further indicate that observation and imitation is an innate social skill because it encourages bonding between the infant and the caregiver through positive social interactions (Jaine et. al, 2013), suggesting that these two learning mechanisms and an infant’s social environment play a crucial role in development. She did this a couple more times and then tried to insert a larger cup into the small opening of the cup. This type of behaviour where infants experiment by repeatedly performing the same actions with some form of variation is typical of 12 to 18mo infants in the tertiary circular reactions sub-stage of Piaget’s theory. It may also indicate that she is self-motivated to initiate different ways of learning. The caregiver began to feed the infant food and she continued to play with the cups in between bites for a while. Every so often the infant exhibited “m-m-m” sounds while eating, which may indicate that she was enjoying her food or trying to asking for more. Her play began to be less active because she started to doze off while eating. Interestingly, the infant still opened her mouth and chewed the food while nodding off periodically. Since infants consolidate their sleep into a night sleep cycle and a daytime nap cycle by 12mo (Middlemiss, 2007), her behaviour may be indicative that she did not receive enough REM sleep because of night wakings or had difficulties self-soothing from the previous night. She soon became frustrated and began to whine when the caregiver tried to continued feeding her, so she tried to climb out of the highchair herself. The caregiver intervened and carried her out to head towards her crib for a nap. Although the infant was tired, she began to cry for approximately 20 minutes until the caregiver went to comfort her by soothing her and rocking her to sleep. This may be a result of operant conditioning, in which the caregiver’s actions positively reinforce the infant’s undesirable behaviour. In this specific case, the infant learned that crying during naptime results in excessive positive attention from her caregiver so that she didn’t have to sleep alone. Structured Observation After the infant’s nap, activities guided by myself and the caregiver were conducted in the living room to assess her cognitive, motor, language and social-emotional development.
I began testing the A-not-B error by placing a plush toy under one of two cloths within the infant’s reach. The toy was first hidden under cloth A for three consecutive trials, and then hidden under cloth B for the fourth trial. Just like most infants who pass the A-not-B error test at approximately 10 months, she correctly identified the location of the toy for each of the four trials. Basing her behaviour on information-processing theory, it suggests that she has a developed schema of object permanence and strong mental representation of objects due to increased working memory capacity. 10mo infants are capable of successfully locating hidden objects as a result of improved attention, inhibitory and motor memory skills over time (Ruffman and Slade, 2005), so her behaviour demonstrates that she may be focusing on achieving the intention of goals by inhibiting previously rewarded motor responses at a younger age. After this task, I asked her to walk towards me with one of her toys. Although she continued to play with her toys at first, she successfully completed the task after some time. This demonstrates that she was able to understand and remember the task that was asked of her even though she was preoccupied with a task of her own interest. I then asked her to find her caregiver who was upstairs.
She used the stair banister to support her ascent, but she scooted down in a sitting position during descent. Although most infants between 13 to 16 months descend the stairs on their bellies with their feet first between, all strategies for ascent and descent relate to the interaction of increased cognitive demands for assessing the situation, coordinating complex locomotor skills, and knowledge of the infant’s own capabilities in order to achieve their goal (Berger et. al, 2007). This interaction between her behaviour and cognition allowed her to choose a strategy that she was most comfortable at the moment. As her actions become more complex with age, so do her thoughts about her abilities and environment, and vice versa. Therefore, simply changing the nature of the task may constrain well-established motor functioning by making the infant temporarily use alternative approaches that better accommodate the task. After this, I gave the infant some crayons and a piece of paper to assess her fine motor skills and cognitive development. Her actions demonstrated that she was able to grasp a crayon and to use it to scribble on a piece of paper. Although her drawing was indecipherable to me, she exhibited the ability to draw within a contained space and to listen to instruction. Children attempt to produce recognizable images at approximately 3 years of age, so her scribbles on the paper are not atypically considering that she is only 15mo. I then asked her questions about her drawing, and later asked her to identify her own body parts and my body parts. She did not use any holophrastic speech besides “yes” and “no” to reply, but she did correctly respond to each question about her drawing, and pointed to the correct body parts when asked to. Her responses demonstrate that she was able to identify where the words start and end through the process of word segmentation with the use of statistical learning, which consequently allows her to associate words with meaning. This also indicates that she has the knowledge and understanding of words she is unable to express by herself with words. As most of her responses were accompanied with arm movements and either a smile or a giggle, it further suggests that she may be trying to communicate much more than she is able to during this stage of development. Towards the end of my structured observation, I left the room only to return with a neutral facial expression and showed no response to any of her actions. This test is known as the still face procedure and it assesses an infant’s abilities to maintain affective regulation. The infant giggled and babbled when I was interacting with her earlier, but gazed into another direction when I was unresponsive to her actions moments after I entered the room expressionless. This gaze aversion behaviour helps infants to engage in self-regulation of emotions and is typical of 12 to 24mo infants. Her behaviour suggested that she was capable of understanding the intersubjectivity needed for social interaction and has the ability to connect my facial and verbal expressions with my feelings. Thus, her gaze aversion was an attempt to regulate her own emotional distress by disengaging from social interaction that was unreciprocated. Contrastingly, her behaviour when I was responsive to her indicated that was also capable of detecting the intentions of their social partners to help them achieve the goals of reciprocated social interaction (Adamanson and Frick, 2003).
Jonsson, B., & von Hofsten, C. (2003). Infants’ ability to track and reach for temporary occluded objects. Developmental Science, 6(1), 86-99.
from the first bar, she quickly swung her feet over to the side for leg support.
Naturalistic observation involves recording subjects' naturally occurring behavior while they are in their natural environment. This experiment revolves around this type of observation. Specifically, it involves the observation of the various human dyads (male-female, male-male, female-female,) social interaction, within in a public environment. Focal points of observation included conversation space (distance between individuals heads,), and body language.
In this assignment I am going to describe a child observation that I have done in a nursery for twenty minutes in a play setting. I will explain the strengths and weaknesses of naturalistic observation through the key developmental milestones based in Mary Sheridan (2005) check-list and provide a theoretical explanation to support the naturalistic observation.
Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond baby talk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review, 27(4), 501-532.
I do think that young infants are more competent than I originally thought they were. I was aware of the reflexes talked about in the first video, Primary Reflexes. I knew that they were born with the abilities such as the sucking, swallowing, walking, babinski, and grasping reflex, which is still pretty incredible. I was really surprised that the two-month-old baby in the video, Infant Looking Time Habituation, she could see the cayenne pepper, control where her eyes aimed, and was able to recognize that it was a repeated object that she had seen over and over again. When the cayenne pepper was placed in front of her she become bored of the familiar object and would look away sooner each time. When the celery salt was placed in front of her
The mother reports remembering her daughter babbling, but says she did not say her first word until much later on than her first child. She said that this was a concern for her and that she discussed it with her pediatrician who did not see any cause for alarm at the time. When t...
A child is considered an infant from the age of 2 to 12 months. From 12 months to 36 months this is considered the toddler years where the cognitive, emotional, and social development is great. The social emotional development occurs during early childhood where children experience different moods as well as expanding their social world by learning more about their emotions and other people. The social emotional development is a child's way of understanding the feelings of others, controlling their own feelings and behaviors and getting along with peers. The key to a successful emotional and social development are positive relationship with trusting and caring adults. The social and emotional development in infants and toddlers can have negative
Deferred and inferred Imitation: The infant also imitates what the adults do especially at the age of 14 -18 months. At the early months of this stage, the child exemplifies deferred imitations, which later
Infants and Children: Prenatal Through Middle Childhood. Pearson/Allyn and Bacon. Cooper, J., Masi, R., & Vick, J. (2009). The 'Standard' of the 'Standard'. Social-emotional Development in Early Childhood.
By understanding and identifying where children are at, they are then able to determine which strategies to help facilitate development in each domain. In Quan’s case, his cognitive skills show that he is able to grasp a variety of concepts and words which is evident through observing his interacts with the toys in the classroom. According to Copple and Bredekamp (2009), “It is helpful to remember that they are not merely functioning less effectively than older children or adults; their narrow focus on a limited amount of information at any given time is actually useful while they are learning so many things so rapidly” (p. 130). By understanding each child’s cognitive development, teachers are then able to create instruction and experiences to facilitate learning and development in those specific cognitive areas. Not only are preschoolers able to interact with materials, but they are also able to, “Use their senses to observe and explore classroom materials and natural phenomena” (Dichtelmiller & Jablon, 2001, p. 118). Due to the importance of providing sensory rich experiences, teachers need to use a variety of hands-on and observation opportunities to help facilitate learning with preschoolers. This is important due to preschoolers mostly learning through their five senses and through their experiences inside and outside the
As children develop, they begin to develop certain traits and behavior that they have picked up from the environment that they have been exposed to. They begin to respond in certain ways based on experiences that have had. Infants in particular are an example of the way these certain behaviors are brought to surface. They will begin to develop either positive or negative perspective towards certain items. One way in which a child develops these perspectives and experiences is through a Behavioral Approach influenced by John Watson. The idea behind it is that a child’s behavior based on their environment (McLeod 1). It is based off of the observation made by the child.
What are the differences between only children and the youngest child of a larger family (ie. Families with four or five children) and are the effects noticeable in adulthood? In most cases, only children and the youngest child are given more than a child in a different position in the Birth Order. The reasons behind the this are very different, the only child is given more because the parents do not have to spread their attention to other children, whereas the youngest receives everything because the parents are tired of fighting once they get to their fourth or fifth child. The other difference between the two group is the most fascinating to see the effects of in adulthood, freedom. Only children have very little freedom, their parents have no other children are able to focus on and often restrict the child’s daily activates. The youngest, on the other hand, has more freedom than they know what to do with. This causes a divide in these group otherwise similar childhood and therefore affects how they will act as adults.
Infant memory is a fairly new concept that has been discovered as time has progressed. Memory is the mental faculty of retaining and recalling past experiences. Infant memory is a lot more complex than adult memory; early findings have detected that infants are capable of retaining memory for a period of time. These discoveries introduced us to different methods of testing infant memory: operant conditioning, visual paired comparison and deferred imitation. Each of these methods shows evidence of some memory retention in infants, demonstrating that infants do have the ability to retain information within infancy period. To understand this, a thorough explanation of one of the research methods (operant conditioning) demonstrates how these procedures
Object permanence is defined as “the knowledge that objects continue to exist even when they are out of view.” (Siegler et al., 2017). There are many views that come with this topic, for instance Piaget, a psychologist, believed that for infants objects permanence is one of the most significant accomplishments. He emphasized this topic during his sensorimotor stage. Some infants grasp the concept quicker while others do not, but generally infants begin to develop the concept at around 8 months of age. A scientific article was done “to examine attentional predictors of search in 5-month-old infants (as measured by the looking A-not-B task), and whether levels of maternal education moderated the effect of the predictors” (Marcovitch et al., 2016). The studies results concluded that 5 month old infants during the A-not-B task appropriately assed object permanence, and that the infants of the mothers that had less