Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
Essays on false memory
Essays on false memories
False memories tie into cognitive psychology
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
Recommended: Essays on false memory
By and large, researchers assert that the customary understanding regarding research and the subsequent views garnered through the research of memory can straightforwardly provide justification for the understanding of false memories (Schacter, 1999). As a starting point it is easy to interpret the understanding of this theoretical paradigm by accepting that the reconstructive nature of memory was grounded in research by Bartlett’s 1932 study (as cited in Gleaves, et. al, 2004) in which he established that an individual’s experiences or occurrences are reconstructed in the light of present views, using available schemas, that is, knowledge structures. In fact, such schemas guide not only the retrieval of events, but even their storage (Straube, …show more content…
2012). Therefore, in regards to the constructivist nature of schema theory, it suggest that while individuals encounter an event or occurrence of a what they believe to be a recognizable situation, whose constituents have been firmly exemplified in an engrained structuralized memory representation (or schema), commonly that configuration produces both factual and fabricated memories in relation to the experience (Schacter, 1999). This is seen to take place simply because, as researchers have noted there are similarities amongst the processes which generate both true and false memories. Thus it can be somewhat confidently established in the schema theory paradigm, by stating that at the level of the processes that construct and regulate them, true and false memories appear to be relatively analogous (Schacter, 1999). Misattributions can be candidly defined as occurrences when memory is attributed to a wrong source (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993).
As such, they have been aptly referred to as source monitoring errors. Throughout research, studies have assembled a tri-part variant understanding regarding how source monitoring can come about, that is to say there are external, internal and reality misattributions in this theoretical paradigm (Johnson et al., 1993). External misattributions are explained as when an individual attributes the source of a genuine experience or occurrence having been attributed to another actual experience or occurrence. Through progressive research, this understanding has been suggested as a viable rationale in regards to the cause of numerous incorrect accusations stemming from inaccurate eyewitness testimony (Lindsay, 2007). This is explained as occurring because rather than choosing the correct perpetrator in police line-ups or police identification photographs it has been found that the witness is prone to choose a recognized individual instead (Lindsay, 2007). Put plainly, the memory of the individual in the police line up or picture is wrongly attributed to the memory of the crime. The understanding of internal source monitoring is defined as the aptitude of discriminating amongst what an individual actually thought and what she or he actually said (Johnson, 1997). There is a high likelihood that individuals could confuse what they think that they …show more content…
may have stated something out loud but in all actuality, never actually spoke. Reality monitoring is seen as involving both internal and external source monitoring errors, as it is an intricate, multifaceted phenomenon (Johnson, 1997). Confabulations are a primary example of reality monitoring errors. Furthermore, individuals diagnosed with Korsakoff syndrome from chronic alcohol abuse or cerebral contusions regularly confabulate (Kopelman, 1999). That is, these individuals construct or generate memories of experiences or incidents which never actually took place, through the usage of preceding experiences and internal narrative, often with a temporal confusions and fragmented spatial context (Kopelman, 1999). What is more, there is evidence to display individuals not afflicted by these circumstances in good on rare occasions may confabulate (Burgess, 1996). This is exemplified through a situation where the individuals were persuaded that some occurrence in reality happened, but in all factuality never did. However, researchers are presently uncertain how exactly individuals which are entirely healthy manufacture confabulations. Additionally, another approach of theories concentrates upon defective encoding of the presented words as a major determining factor in the production of false memory within what has been labeled as the Fuzzy-trace theory (FTT; Brainerd, & Reyna, 1998).
Brainerd, & Reyna, (1998) propose that human memories are stored as two opponent traces. Verbatim traces collect item-specific surface characteristics of information, and gist traces stock meanings of the information being processed. Therefore, in this conceptual classification, false memories are generated generally because of a dependence upon the gist traces because the verbatim traces lose strength and ultimately wane, which makes them less obtainable in memory (Brainerd, & Reyna, 1998). This ultimately suggests that false memories take place as a result of an incapability to isolate both gist and verbatim traces at the initial encoding
phase. What is also important to note is that in this interpretation the FTT paradigm provides a potential explanation, if not accurate interpretation for the mistaken retrieval of the gist words which are not presented in the DRM false memory experiments; with the gist being the theme of the list presented (Brainerd, & Reyna, 1998). Taken together, both theories suggest that because of the thorough and meticulous semantic encoding process in human memory, the construction of false memories is simply an inadvertent end result. Research conducted experimentally regarding source-monitoring errors frequently cites theories which accentuates the role of associative processes in creating false memories, known as activation monitoring. For instance, in the Deese–Roediger and McDermott theory (DRM) (Roediger & McDermott, 1995), false memories in these studies were induced by the associative priming procedure created for free recall by Deese (1959) and expanded by Roediger and McDermott (1995) in order to demonstrate that false memories can be produced in a controlled environment. In this paradigm participants are shown a list of semantically interrelated words and in this procedure, a significant proportion of individuals falsely recall words which are not present during the encoding phase. These words are seen as semantically associated with a critical lure which subsequently appears to activate an associate word when prompted with a list of semantically interrelated words (Roediger & McDermott, 1995). This is interpreted as an instance of internal source monitoring where the individuals’ source monitoring systems inaccuracy or malfunction in discriminating amongst what words are in fact presented and the critical word activation (Watson, Bunting, Poole, & Conway, 2005). In regards to suggestibility it is seen to be to some degree analogous to misattribution, although there is the addition of overt suggestion (Loftus, & Palmer, 1974). This theoretical understanding is often presented in conjunction with what is labeled the misinformation effect; as it establishes that suggestible information along with information consisting of greater detailed an individual obtains following the creation of the actual memory can replace or transform the old memory (Loftus, 2005). The misinformation effect, as established and explained by researchers (see Loftus, & Palmer, 1974; Loftus, & Pickrell, 1995; Loftus, 2005), in the original lab controlled experimental paradigm, is generated through a basic tri-part progression which is comprised of the presentation of the original events, interceding events projected to mislead the participant, and a memory test (Pickrell, 2008, p.441). As a result, suggestions or misleading post-event information (MPI) has the potentiality to generate alterations of older memories. Furthermore, memories are for the most part, extremely apt to distortion at the retrieval phase of memory. This is markedly apparent within the aforementioned misinformation paradigm, in the understanding that the research suggest that the retrieval cue employed for recall may subsequently have a prominent effect upon the actual details of the memory which is retrieved (Loftus, & Palmer, 1974). Essentially, the prominent studies and theoretical models in this area of research focus paint a picture of human memory as malleable and prone to errors. What is more, based upon the research false memories and the corruptions associated with them seem to originate issues originating at specific points in the creation of memories encoding, consolidation, and retrieval (Straube, 2012).
This paper will consider eye witness testimony and its place in convicting accused criminals. Psychology online (2013) defines “eye witness testimony” as a statement from a person who has witnessed a crime, and is capable of communicating what they have seen, to a court of law under oath. Eye witness testimonies are used to convict accused criminals due to the first hand nature of the eye witnesses’ observations. There are however many faults within this system of identification. Characteristics of the crime is the first issue that will be discussed in this paper, and the flaws that have been identified. The second issue to be discussed will be the stress impact and the inability to correctly identify the accused in a violent or weapon focused crime. The third issue to be discussed is inter racial identification and the problems faced when this becomes a prominent issue. The fourth issue will be time lapse, meaning, the time between the crime and the eye witness making a statement and how the memory can be misconstrued in this time frame. To follow this will be the issue of how much trust jurors-who have no legal training-put on to the eye witness testimony, which may be faltered. This paper references the works of primarily Wells and Olsen (2003) and Rodin (1987) and Schmechel et al. (2006) it will be argued that eye witness testimony is not always accurate, due to many features; inter racial identification, characteristics of the crime, response latency, and line up procedures therefore this paper will confirm that eyewitness testimonies should not be utilised in the criminal ju...
Roediger III, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating False Memories: Remembering Words Not Presented in Lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cogntion, 21, 803-814.
(Kennedy & Haygood, 1992; Williams & Loftus, 1994), which is worrying considering the growing and substantial body of evidence from laboratory studies, field studies, and the criminal justice system supporting the conclusion that eyewitnesses frequently make mistakes (Cutler & Penrod, 1995; Huff, 1987; Huff, Rattner, & Sagarin, 1986; Innocence Project, 2009; Wells, Small, Penrod, Malpass, Fulero, & Brimacombe, 1998). According to a number of studies, eyewitness misidentifications are the most common cause of wrongful convictions (Huff, Rattner, & Sagarin, 1986; Wells et al., 1998; Yarmey, 2003) and, through the use of forensic DNA testing, have been found to account for more convictions of innocent individuals than all other factors combined (Innocence Project, 2009; Wells, Memon, & Penrod, 2006).
Humans have an incredible capability for thinking and memory. We can remember events from our past, for our future, and of things that have no relative meaning to ourselves. These memories can be traced back to different systems of our brains through a process of encoding, storage, and retrieval. As part of the retrieval process, memories can be remembered with or without their sources. As research has found, our memories are not labeled or tagged with their origin (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay 1993). Because of this, our memory has developed a process called source monitoring. This is how we link our memories to the source that they developed from, usually using specific characteristics and general knowledge of the memory. For example, source monitoring includes identifying who told you something, whether or not you saw an event in real life, the time of the event and whether you told something to your friend or only thought about telling it. The source-monitoring framework for the process involved in pinpointing the origin of information by Johnson and colleagues, explains both vertical and distorted memory with a common set of principles. First, a specific memory consists of specific characteristics including spatial, temporal, and perceptual details. Secondly, the memories can differ in characteristics that can be used to find the origin. More extensive source monitoring can involve beliefs about memory and cognition as well as retrieving more information from memory and finding the source of the memory given these beliefs, other specific characteristics or general knowledge (Johnson et al. 1993). Sometimes these beliefs aren't always accurate. Because some people may be influenced by their personal ideologies during retriev...
During the identification and prosecution of a suspect, eyewitnesses are the most important. Eyewitness testimony needs to be reliable as it can have serious implications to the perceived guilt or innocence of a defendant. Unfortunately, the reliability of eyewitness testimony is questionable because there is a high number of eyewitness misidentification. Rattner (1988) studied 205 cases and concluded that eyewitness misidentification was the factor most often associated with wrongful conviction (52%). Eyewitness testimony can be affected by many factors. A substantial literature demonstrates own group biases in eyewitness testimony. For example, the own-race bias, in which people are better at recognizing faces of their own race versus another
Eyewitness identification and testimony play a huge role in the criminal justice system today, but skepticism of eyewitnesses has been growing. Forensic evidence has been used to undermine the reliability of eyewitness testimony, and the leading cause of false convictions in the United States is due to misidentifications by eyewitnesses. The role of eyewitness testimony in producing false confessions and the factors that contribute to the unreliability of these eyewitness testimonies are sending innocent people to prison, and changes are being made in order to reform these faulty identification procedures.
Steffens, M., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2007). False memories: Phenomena, theories, and implications. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Journal Of Psychology, 215(1), 12-24. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.12
There has been considerable debate worldwide, regarding the accuracy of eyewitness testimony in the criminal justice system. Particularly, arguments have surrounded wrongful convictions that have resulted from incorrect eyewitness evidence (Areh, 2011; Howitt, 2012; Nelson, Laney, Bowman-Fowler, Knowles, Davis & Loftus, 2011). The purpose of this essay is to consider psychological research about the accuracy of eyewitness testimony and its placement in the criminal justice system. Firstly, this essay will define how eyewitnesses and their testimonies are used within the criminal justice system and the current debate surrounding its usage. Secondly, the impact of post-identification feedback will be used to show the affect on the confidence of a witness. Thirdly, studies around gender related differences will show how a witnesses gender can affect memory recall and accuracy. Fourthly, empirical studies will be used to highlight how a psychological experience called change blindness can cause mistakes in eyewitness identification. Finally, the effect of cross-examination will be used to explore the impact on eyewitness accuracy. It will be argued, that eyewitness testimony is not accurate and highly subjective, therefore, the criminal justice system must reduce the impact that eyewitness testimony is allowed to have. Developing better policies and procedures to avoid wrongful convictions by misled judges and jury members can do this.
The article How to Tell If a Particular Memory Is True or False by Daniel M. Bernstein and Elizabeth F. Loftus, addresses the various techniques used by cognitive scientists and other researchers in hopes of distinguishing true from false memories. For this article Loftus and Bernstein, memory researchers, chose to discuss the different methods currently used, rather than trying to find new ways to tell if a particular memory is true or false. Their findings in these three different approaches are very interesting, and leads us to think critically of the veracity of true and false memories.
Similar studies were done to a different set of college students and they tended to have the same results. After giving as much detail about each memory, the students were interviewed about what they may have written done about what they had remembered. During the last part of the experiment, each of the students were debriefed and asked to guess which memory they believed was false.
Eyewitness testimony is defined as, “an area of research that investigates the accuracy of memory following an accident, crime, or other significant event, and the types of errors that are commonly made in such situations.” Much emphasis is placed on the accuracy of eyewitness testimony as often-inaccurate eyewitness testimony can have serious consequences leading to wrong convictions. Eyewitness testimony is a powerful tool within any field, particularly that of justice, as it is a readily accepted form of evidence that allows for convictions. However, Tests conducted by Loftus have shown an enormous swing from a non-guilty verdict, to guilty within the same case, simply through the introduction of an eyewitness. This alone displays the importance of eyewitness testimony, and accentuates the theory that jurors tend to over believe, or at least rely heavily on such accounts.
In recent years there has been a hot debate between "repressed" vs. "false" memories. Neurobiological studies show that both suppression and recall and the creation of false memories are possible. This paper evaluates the evidence but forth by both sides of the controversy and concludes that both are feasible and separate phenomenon, which occur at significant rates in our society.
One of the most interesting phenomenon related to memory is memory distortions. One way in which they occur is through suggestibility, where people begin to remember false experiences if researchers suggested to them that they experienced it (Sternberg and Sternberg, 2012). In real-life situations, this is caused in part by memory being constructive “in that prior experiences affects how we recall things and what we actually recall from memory” (Sternberg and Sternberg, 2012). People’s prior experiences, including their bias and expectations, may influence how they experience false memory formations; the formation of false memories is also affected by several possible factors, one of which may be sleep deprivation (Frenda, Patihis, Loftus,
Memory is the tool we use to learn and think. We all use memory in our everyday lives. Memory is the mental faculty of retaining and recalling past experiences. We all reassure ourselves that our memories are accurate and precise. Many people believe that they would be able to remember anything from the event and the different features of the situation. Yet, people don’t realize the fact that the more you think about a situation the more likely the story will change. Our memories are not a camcorder or a camera. Our memory tends to be very selective and reconstructive.
According to Sternberg (1999), memory is the extraction of past experiences for information to be used in the present. The retrieval of memory is essential in every aspect of daily life, whether it is for academics, work or social purposes. However, many often take memory for granted and assume that it can be relied on because of how realistic it appears in the mind. This form of memory is also known as flashbulb memory. (Brown and Kulik, 1977). The question of whether our memory is reliably accurate has been shown to have implications in providing precise details of past events. (The British Psychological Association, 2011). In this essay, I would put forth arguments that human memory, in fact, is not completely reliable in providing accurate depictions of our past experiences. Evidence can be seen in the following two studies that support these arguments by examining episodic memory in humans. The first study is by Loftus and Pickrell (1995) who found that memory can be modified by suggestions. The second study is by Naveh-Benjamin and Craik (1995) who found that there is a predisposition for memory to decline with increasing age.