Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
Essay on false memories
False memory
Conclusions on false memories
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
Recommended: Essay on false memories
Whose Memory is it Anyway?:
False Memories and their Potential Effects on Police Interrogations
Justin “Ty” Mosier
Middle Tennessee State University
Whose Memory is it Anyway?:
False Memories and their Potential Effects on Police Interrogations
Many decisions are made every day based on the films being played in one’s head. However, how certain are we that these memories are film like and do not change? How would one know that the memory has changed? In studies, memories have been shown to not have constancy. They have also shown that memories can be manipulated and false memories can be implanted into your mind. Memories have the potential to be influenced by police interrogations, and it can lead to someone being falsely imprisoned.
…show more content…
The first way that memories are affected is by using leading questions (Loftus, 2003). Leading questions are questions that are phrased to produce an expected answer. A great example of this would be from a study conducted by Elizabeth Lotus, who used leading questions in investigations into car accidents in an attempt to provoke an altered memory. “I began showing people films of traffic accidents and questioning them in various ways. The question “Did one see the broken headlight?” led to more false reports of a broken headlight than the same question asked with the verb hit. “How fast were the cars going when they smashed into each other?” led to higher estimates of speed than a more neutral question that used the verb hit. Moreover, the “smashed” question led more people to later falsely claim that they had seen broken glass when there was none. My early papers concluded that leading questions could contaminate or distort a witness’s memory” (Loftus, 2003, P. 868). Loftus explains how using leading questions can persuade an interviewee into saying things that were not completely true. These simple questions can have dramatic effects on someone who thinks they are telling the truth, while their statements are often filled with the opposite. These are very small details that could lead to a big impact on eye witness testimonies and have other implications outside of a court room. Memories can also be affected by the practice of feeding misinformation to unsuspecting individuals.
These techniques usually involve giving misleading information about the topic to an experimental group without providing this information to the control group. What has been observed is that misled participants usually report the incorrect stimuli over the correct. Participants will often incorporate these incorrect stimuli into their memories, while those participants who did not receive incorrect stimuli usually report more accurate responses (Loftus, 2003). Effects such as these can carry on long after the initial exposure. “Misinformation can sway people when they see biased media coverage about some event that they may have experienced themselves” (Loftus, 2003). Loftus says that misinformation can leave someone vulnerable to biased information.
Another way memories can lose ownership is through the implantation of memories. The most common way to achieve that is through the familial informant false-narrative procedure. In this method, the researcher will asks parents for information about their children’s childhood. During the experiment, a true memory and a false memory will be presented. After a few interviews, researchers are successful in convincing some interviewees that the false events that were implanted actually happened (Loftus,
…show more content…
2003). If the researchers encounter the possibility that events could have actually happened, thus confounding the research, another technique can be employed. In this method, the researcher knows exactly what happened on a certain day, because they witnessed the events. “If one as a researcher know precisely what happened that day and one lead the participant to “remember” doing something else, one have fairly good evidence that one have created a false report. This strategy was first adopted by Goff and Roediger (1998) and later modified by my colleague and me (Thomas & Loftus, 2002). In one study, participants sat in front of a large table filled with numerous objects. They heard a series of statements (e.g., “flip the coin”) and then had to perform or imagine performing the called-for actions. The next time they came to the lab, there were no objects in front of them, but they simply imagined that they performed various actions. In a final session, their memory for what they did that first day was tested. After a few imaginations, participants occasionally remembered performing actions that they had not performed. They falsely claimed that they did things that would have been common (e.g., roll the dice), but they also claimed that they did things that would have been rather bizarre or unusual, such as “rub the chalk on oner head” or “kiss a plastic frog” (Loftus, 2003, p. 869). Loftus details how testing memory implantation can happen without the use of a familial informant. These results show that implantation still occurred under these conditions. The occurrence of false memories has been detailed above. How these memories affect people is equally interesting. Since everyday memories are used to make decisions, decision making can be influenced by false memories, as well. Loftus attempted a study that showed the effects of false memories on decisions. “We induced participants to believe that when they were children they got sick eating hard-boiled eggs (or, for other participants, that they got sick eating dill pickles). We accomplished this mental feat by gathering data from the participants and plying them with false feedback. We told them that a sophisticated computer program had analyzed their data and determined that they had had one of these “sick” experiences as a child. We found that those given the “dill pickle” feedback became more confident that they had had the experience as a child and those given the “hard-boiled egg” feedback became more confident of that experience. But would the increase in belief translate into subsequent behavior change? Would they, for example, avoid these foods when given the opportunity to eat them? To find out, we gave participants a “Party Behavior” questionnaire. They imagined themselves at a large barbeque and had to indicate which foods they would like to eat. Those who were seduced by the dill pickle feedback reported being less likely to want to eat pickles, whereas those who fell for the egg feedback reported being less likely to want to eat eggs” (Loftus, 2003, p. 870). Loftus showed how false memories can affect decisions about life. The subjects were less likely to eat the food from the affected memory. These are just food choices, but the implications of these studies can be seen further. The impacts of memory implantation are measureable in many ways. Another impact is seen with the memory implantation of crimes. In a study by Julia Shaw and Stephen Porter, they were able to get 70% of interviewees to show signs of having false memories of crimes. In the paper, the author stated that not only could their subjects create these false memories, but they were very rich and detailed (Shaw, 2015). The subjects incorrectly admitted that they had committed crimes against other people that had not occurred. In fact, they had never even had contact with the police. This is just a small peak into what could be happening in police interrogations. Almost everyone has encountered a situation where they were not completely certain that their own memory could be trusted.
“Did I turn off the coffee pot before we left?” Memory distrust is another way that false memories can have impact life. Guddjonsson defined memory distrust as “a condition were people develop profound distrust of their own memory recollections, as a result of which they are particularly susceptible to relying on external cues and suggestions” (Gudjonsson, 2014, p. 28). He is inferring that when we are not certain about a memory, we can become vulnerable to outside influence over that memory. Usually memory distrust is triggered by an event that leads to people contemplating whether or not they actually did something. This often leads to acceptance of the false memory. Schacter has identified seven flaws in human memory that lead to mistrust of memories. Those flaws include transience, absent-mindedness, blocking, misattribution, suggestibility, bias, and persistence. These seven flaws working together or independently, can cause memories to become vulnerable to change (Gudjonsson, 2014). These seven flaws play a vital role in the acceptance of the false
memory. In a study interviewing people who had falsely confessed to committing crimes, they found that people actually believed that they had committed the crime. The study points to five steps that occurs in this process. A trigger event happens, in this case the interrogation, which plants the idea in their mind that they did it. Secondly, plausibility sets in; the person of interest begins wondering if they could have committed the crime. After that, Acceptance comes into play. This happens after questioning and usually during isolation from human contact. Next is reconstruction; after someone accepts that they have committed the crime, they begin to create their own memory of the event- usually with assistance of police evidence. The final step that they argue is resolution, this is when the person has realized they have a false memory. Unfortunately, not every person with a false memory gets to this point (Gudjonsson, 2014). However, it has been show if you warn someone about these effects, they can become resistant to these memory changes (Loftus, 2003). People often say that memories are subjective. However, that seems like an excuse not to measure those memories and how they affect our lives. Whether memories are subjective or otherwise is not the point. Memories are definitely qualitative in nature but that surely does not mean that they cannot be quantified. Memories are not written in stone and they seem to interact with all facets of our lives. Or as Elizabeth Loftus said, “Who we are may be shaped by our memories, but our memories are shaped by who we are and what we have been led to believe (Loftus, 2003). These studies point to memories not being perfect replications of past events. In fact, it is very possible to push someone to believe that they did something contrary to their memories.
7% of group A said “yes” and 15% of group B said “Yes” to an item
After reviewing the article “Inside Interrogation: The Lie, The Bluff, and False Confessions”, it became very evident the huge problem with interrogations and false confessions in the criminal justice system is with false confession. Jennifer T. Perillo and Saul M. Kassin crafted three distinct experiments to try and better understand false confessions and how trues the actual numbers in real life are. What Perillo and Kassin were trying to prove is that “the bluff technique should elicit confessions from perpetrators but not from innocents” (Perillo, Kassin 2010). What is called the “Bluff Technique” is an interrogation technique that uses a sort of threat or hint that there is certain proof that a person will think is more of a promise for
People face ethical dilemmas every day. But it is perhaps, most prevalent in the law enforcement profession. Law enforcement officers face ethical dilemmas constantly. Some of the ethical issues that police face each day are: racial profiling, officer discretion, police officer loyalty, police officer abuse, and interrogatory deception. This paper will discuss the purpose of interrogatory deception, ways in which it is used, some of the current debates over the practice, and a landmark ruling in the Miranda case of 1966 which attempted to cease the use of intimidation and coercion practices of the police.
Steffens, M., & Mecklenbräuker, S. (2007). False memories: Phenomena, theories, and implications. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie/Journal Of Psychology, 215(1), 12-24. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.12
The study of the misinformation effect dates back to the 1970s with an experiment by Elizabeth Loftus and coworkers (Loftus, Miller, & Burns, Semantic Integration of Verbal Information Into a Visual Memory, 1978). The experiment involved participants that were shown a series of slides with a car that stops at a stop sign and then turns and hits a pedestrian. The participants were then asked if another car passed the red Datsun while it was stopped at the red stop sign, this would have been the control group. Another set of participants were asked the same question with the words “stop sign” replaced by the words “yield sign,” which ...
The article How to Tell If a Particular Memory Is True or False by Daniel M. Bernstein and Elizabeth F. Loftus, addresses the various techniques used by cognitive scientists and other researchers in hopes of distinguishing true from false memories. For this article Loftus and Bernstein, memory researchers, chose to discuss the different methods currently used, rather than trying to find new ways to tell if a particular memory is true or false. Their findings in these three different approaches are very interesting, and leads us to think critically of the veracity of true and false memories.
In class this week we saw cases where our memory fails when recalling information in the past and even seeing changes in real time in front of our eyes. Misinformation is when someone gives information that is incorrect without the intent of giving the incorrect information. This is more likely to happen in situations where the new information would fit someone’s schema of the situation. In a crime example, we can say that a robber has a knife in his hand and pointed it at someone, someone in the situation may have seen a gun instead of the knife and give that as a description. It is the same when someone misreads a word or letter as another in a license plate. It is not on purpose but it is because our brains use shortcuts to link events together to save time. In class we discussed why the new false memory might take hold as if it was the true memory. We said that since the new misinformation was more recent, it would be more easily remembered and projected into the memory. This blocking theory can be used to explain why when someone says they saw a gun at a crime scene to the other witnesses, the other witnesses may be more likely to say that there was a gun and not a knife
There have been several experiments done to try to prove that false memories can indeed be formed. One experiment, for example, was tried with a 14-year-old boy. The boy was told four memories, one of which was falsely constructed but similar to that of a true memory. The memories that were suggested took place when the boy was about 10 years younger. As the false memory was retold to him, he was asked to explain in detail what he had remembered from that event. Surprisingly, he claimed to remember the event, even though it was falsely created by the interviewer and his brother, and went on to explain what he remember to have happened, details and all. After collecting everything he had said about the four memories, he was told that one of the suggested memories was made-up and he was asked to guess which one it may have been. When he couldn’t decide which one it was, he was told that it was in fact the memory of getting lost in a store. He was confused and had trouble believing the truth.
In recent years there has been a hot debate between "repressed" vs. "false" memories. Neurobiological studies show that both suppression and recall and the creation of false memories are possible. This paper evaluates the evidence but forth by both sides of the controversy and concludes that both are feasible and separate phenomenon, which occur at significant rates in our society.
Eyewitnesses are often needed to help solve a crime and find the identity of a culprit. Eyewitness testimony is used in the legal system and refers to people recalling an event they have witnessed. However research that I will mention in this essay, has shown that we should not rely on only eyewitnesses to identify a culprit. “I saw it with my own two eyes” is a common expression used to underline that someone has witnessed something and can recall it exactly like it happened. Memories are commonly mistaken for exact scripts of previous events. What most people do not seem to realise, is that memories can be reformed. This means, that you might think you remember something accurately, but it turns out that it actually did not even happen.
Memory is one of the most critical parts of cognition. It is important because it is involved in almost every aspect of cognition including problem solving, decision making, attention, and perception. Because of this importance, people rely on one’s memory to make important decisions. The value of one’s memory in this society is so high that it is used as evidence to either save one’s life or kill one’s life during murder trials. But as many of the cognitive psychologists know, human’s memory can cause many errors. One of these errors is false memory which is either remembering events that never happened or remembering events differently from the actual event. This finding of false memory raised big interests among psychologists and general public and many researches were done in order to find more about the false memory. The constructive approach to memory, which states that memory is constructed by person based on what really happened in addition to person’s other knowledge, experiences, and expectations, supports the idea of false memory. Just like what constructive approach to memory states, the false memory can be created by person’s knowledge, common biases, and suggestions. The present study was done in order to demonstrate one methodology that biases people to create and recall false memories. The present study is based on Deese’s experiment in 1959 and also on Roediger and McDermott’s experiment in 1995. The participants will be presented with sequence of words visually, and then they would have to classify a set of words as either in the sequence or not in the sequence. Our hypothesis is that people will create false memories and recall distractor words that are related to the sequence of words presented significantly m...
Memory is the tool we use to learn and think. We all use memory in our everyday lives. Memory is the mental faculty of retaining and recalling past experiences. We all reassure ourselves that our memories are accurate and precise. Many people believe that they would be able to remember anything from the event and the different features of the situation. Yet, people don’t realize the fact that the more you think about a situation the more likely the story will change. Our memories are not a camcorder or a camera. Our memory tends to be very selective and reconstructive.
The findings of this research would reinforce findings of previous research specifically in relation to the robustness of false memories. The experiment undertaken by students will also be helpful to the study of false memories specifically to test the reproducibility of this type of experiment.
According to Sternberg (1999), memory is the extraction of past experiences for information to be used in the present. The retrieval of memory is essential in every aspect of daily life, whether it is for academics, work or social purposes. However, many often take memory for granted and assume that it can be relied on because of how realistic it appears in the mind. This form of memory is also known as flashbulb memory. (Brown and Kulik, 1977). The question of whether our memory is reliably accurate has been shown to have implications in providing precise details of past events. (The British Psychological Association, 2011). In this essay, I would put forth arguments that human memory, in fact, is not completely reliable in providing accurate depictions of our past experiences. Evidence can be seen in the following two studies that support these arguments by examining episodic memory in humans. The first study is by Loftus and Pickrell (1995) who found that memory can be modified by suggestions. The second study is by Naveh-Benjamin and Craik (1995) who found that there is a predisposition for memory to decline with increasing age.
People fail to notice when they are presented with something different from what they originally chose and tend to come up with explanations as to why they picked that specific choice. In this research paper, Cochran, Greenspan, Bogart, and Loftus discuss how choice blindness can lead to distorted eyewitness memories. In their experiment, the studied if the participants in their research realized modifications to their memory reports and if these changes could possibly effect the participants’ memory. Cochran, Greenspan, Bogart, and Loftus conducted two different experiments. Experiment 1 was constructed on two self-sourced vs. other-sourced between participants and two misinformation vs. control within participants. They had participants watch a slideshow that showed a woman intermingling with three other characters and one of them steals her wallet. Then they completed a personality measure in 15 minute retention interval which was followed by questions about their memories from the slideshow. After, they were given another 15 minute retention interval and then shown their responses to the memory question, but three of their responses were revised. According to Cochran, Greenspan, Bogart, and Loftus (2016), “experiment 1 demonstrated that when witnesses were exposed to altered versions of their own memory reports for episodic details of an event, their memories changed to be consistent with