In the summer of 1984, 22 - year old Jennifer Thompson’s life drastically changed when a man broke into her apartment, held a knife to her throat and raped her. During the attack, Thompson was determined to stay alert; she studied his face, his voice and any other details that could be used to help identify the perpetrator. Her intention was to survive, and with the information from the attack she planned to help the police catch her rapist. After a composite sketch, photo identification, physical line-up identification and trial, Thompson identified Ronald cotton as her attacker. Her testimony and memory alone was enough to sentence Cotton to life in prison. After a few years in prison, Cotton met a man named Bobby Poole, who had previously …show more content…
boasted to other inmates that he had committed the crime for which cotton was convicted. Even after seeing Poole in a courtroom, Thompson testified twice against Cotton. After 11 years of incarceration, DNA evidence eventually revealed that Cotton was in fact innocent, and instead, Poole was the culprit. This case in particular raises concerns about the reliability of eyewitness testimony and highlights some important errors in eyewitness identification procedures. Research has shown that false eyewitness identification is one of the primary reasons for the conviction of innocent people (Wells et al., 1998). The aim of this essay is to investigate the procedural flaws that might have contributed to the mistaken identity of Ronald Cotton and examine what procedural changes could be implemented to improve the reliability of eyewitness testimony. The Ronald Cotton case is just one example of what could go drastically wrong when court cases rely on human recollection, often leading to a wrongful conviction.
A leading researcher, Elizabeth Loftus reported that members of the jury tend to trust or rely heavily on eyewitness reports especially when the victim has witnessed the crime with their own eyes. However, research has shown that eyewitness testimony is often unreliable and statistics indicate that there are many wrongful convictions every year as a result of unreliable eyewitness testimony (Loftus, 1986; Wade, Green & Nash, 2010). There are a number of different reasons for why false identification can occur during a criminal case, many of which came into play during the Ronald Cotton case. For example, in some cases perception of what occurred is often impaired and the witness’s memory of the event is sometimes faulty. Other common causes of eyewitness error are often due to external factors such as the procedures that police use to obtain the identification of the perpetrator. These factors include suggestive questioning and line-up biases, which have been known to affect the confidence of one’s decision, and in turn, affect the accuracy of the identification process (Wells & Olson,
2002). Several variables have been known to make police identification process more or less reliable and these factors often affect the accuracy of eyewitness identifications (Wells & Olson, 2003). These procedural mechanisms are known as system variables and they refer to factors over which the justice system has some control. Research has primarily focused on four factors, namely the type of line up used, the content of the line-up, communications with the witness before and after the identification process and the behaviour of the line-up administrator. Ronald Cotton’s wrongful conviction is likely to have resulted from a variety of these factors. Firstly, one of the initial problems encountered in a criminal case are the procedures used when conducting a line-up. The moment in which an eyewitness views a line up and identifies a possible criminal suspect can be significant enough to obtain a conviction (Well et al., 1998). A common problem with police line-ups is in some cases the actual culprit might not be included. Research has indicated that when the real perpetrator is not in the photo line-up or physical line up, the witness tends to choose the line-up member who most resembles the actual culprit. This effect is more likely to occur in simultaneous line-ups, where people tend to engage in relative judgement (Wells, 2006). Therefore, line-ups in which several suspects are presented next to each other often encourages witnesses to make decisions about which person looks most like the perpetrator rather than making judgements about whether the culprit is actually present. This type of line-up was used in the Ronald Cotton case, as more than one suspect was presented at the same time, which may have increased the risk of Cotton being wrongfully convicted especially since the actual perpetrator was not present in both line-ups (Wells, 1984). It is possible that Thompson might have assumed that at least one of the six men shown in the photo line-up was the actual culprit and may have been unaware of the possibility that the perpetrator might not be present in both the photo line-up and the physical line-up. As a result, Thompson may have chosen Cotton because he was the person who most resembles Poole. Therefore, without Poole in the line-up, Cotton was the person in jeopardy. Research findings from several different studies provide support for the relative judgement effect. For example, failure to provide instructions to the eyewitness that indicate that the perpetrator might not be in the line-up can increase the risk of an eyewitness to choose someone from the line-up regardless of whether the actual perpetrator is present (Malpass & Devine, 1981). Even when instructions are provided, research has shown that eyewitnesses still tend to use relative judgments (Wells, 1993). Additionally, a study by Dunning and Stern (1994) reported that witnesses who engage in relative comparison processes or an elimination processes when attempting to identify a suspect are more likely to have made a mistaken identification compared to those who report that the face “just popped out”. Therefore, accurate witnesses who correctly identified the culprit are more likely to automatically recognise the perpetrator, whereas inaccurate witnesses are more likely to use comparison and elimination strategies. This issue may have been present in the Ronald Cotton case, as Thompson, with some difficulty eventually chose Cotton after studying the pictures in the photo line-up for 5 minutes. Recognition memory is quite rapid and therefore, Thompson should have been able to identify cotton within a few seconds, suggesting that it was quite likely that she was doing something other than reliable recognition memory. Based on this evidence, psychologists have called on the legal system to implement certain procedural changes in an attempt to reduce the number of wrongful convictions (Wells et al., 1998). In particular, some psychologists argue that sequential line-ups should be used over the traditional simultaneous line-ups as a way of reducing the number of false identifications (Wells et al., 1998; Lindsay et al., 1991). In a sequential line-up eyewitnesses can only view one suspect at a time and for each suspect they are asked whether or not they recognise the person. This process continues until a person from the line-up is positively identified as the perpetrator. As a result, in this type of line-up, eyewitnesses tend to rely only on their memory to make the judgement and therefore this eliminates the tendency for a witness to select a person who most resembles their assailant. Research has shown that fewer errors are made during the identification process when a sequential line-up is administrated as opposed to a simultaneous line-up (Steblay, Dysart, Fulero & Lindsay, 2001). The use a sequential line-up in the Ronald Cotton case, may have discouraged Thompson from making comparisons among line-up members and encourage her to compare each line up member based only on her memory of the perpetrator. During the physical line-up, Thompson was very careful when trying to identify the perpetrator. She viewed the line-up for a while and eventually informed the detective that the assailant was either number 4 or 5 and asked to have the line-up repeated. Eventually she stated that Cotton “looks most like the perpetrator”. Therefore, it evident that Thompson engaged in a process of elimination and compared the line-up members to each other to determine which one looked most like the assailant. Since Poole was not in the photo line-up or physical line-up she chose the person who looked the most familiar. If a sequential line-up had been administrated during the identification process, then Thompson would be forced to base her decision on how well each person matches the information stored in her memory about the perpetrator. As a result, she may have been less likely to identify Cotton as the attacker. Although many psychologists recommend the sequential line-up procedure, there is still an ongoing debate about which line-up procedure should be administrated, as the conditions under which sequential line-ups are superior to simultaneous line-ups are still not fully understood (Surrett, Malpass & Tredoux, 2006).
On June 9th 1959 near Clinton, Ontario 14-year-old Steven Truscott gave his classmate 12-year-old Lynne Harper a ride on his bike from their school down to Highway 8 (Ontario Justice Education Network Timeline of Events for the Steven Truscott Case). This sole event would be the one to change his life forever. The next day Lynne’s body was discovered near Lawson’s bush (close to the area in which he dropped her off) where she had been strangled, sexually assaulted and subsequently killed. That day Constable Hobbs conducted lengthy seven-hour interview on young Steven Truscott in which he asked him a number
Gail Miller was a 22-year-old nursing assistant living in Saskatoon. She was found in an alley way between 6:45 and 7:30am on January 31st 1969. She had been raped, stabbed twelve times and left for dead. The rape was found to have occurred after she died. The police had little evidence; few clues had been left behind. There had been other attacks in the same area. Authorities tried to suppress the information that linked the Miller rape and murder to the two other assaults.
Jennifer Thompson-Cannino was raped at knife point in her apartment. She was able to escape and identify Ronald Cotton as her attacker. The detective conducting the lineup told Jennifer that she had done great, confirming to her that she had chosen the right suspect. Eleven years later, DNA evidence proved that the man Jennifer Identified, Ronald Cotton was innocent and wrongfully convicted. Instead, Bobby Poole was the real perpetrator. Sadly, there are many other cases of erroneous convictions. Picking cotton is a must read for anybody because it educates readers about shortcomings of eyewitness identification, the police investigative process and the court system.
In a compelling story of forgiveness and moving on, Jennifer Thompson and Ronald Cotton shared their journey with the Ferris community. Thompson took the audience to a setting in July 29, 1984, by recounting the night of her rape and the measures she went to memorize the face of evil. Thompson, with utmost certainty, declared Cotton her rapist after hours of police interrogations, forming sketches, and a lineup. Cotton was sentenced to life in prison but was at Ferris to share his side of the story and how DNA testing changed his life (Picking Cotton). The events of 1984 were told from two separate accounts, with distinguished parallels discussing the importance of DNA, misidentification, and coercion from police. The rape of Thompson not only affected herself, but Cotton as well.
In the magic of the mind author Dr. Elizabeth loftus explains how a witness’s perception of an accident or crime is not always correct because people's memories are often imperfect. “Are we aware of our minds distortions of our past experiences? In most cases, the answer is no.” our minds can change the way we remember what we have seen or heard without realizing it uncertain witnesses “often identify the person who best matches recollection
This paper will consider eye witness testimony and its place in convicting accused criminals. Psychology online (2013) defines “eye witness testimony” as a statement from a person who has witnessed a crime, and is capable of communicating what they have seen, to a court of law under oath. Eye witness testimonies are used to convict accused criminals due to the first hand nature of the eye witnesses’ observations. There are however many faults within this system of identification. Characteristics of the crime is the first issue that will be discussed in this paper, and the flaws that have been identified. The second issue to be discussed will be the stress impact and the inability to correctly identify the accused in a violent or weapon focused crime. The third issue to be discussed is inter racial identification and the problems faced when this becomes a prominent issue. The fourth issue will be time lapse, meaning, the time between the crime and the eye witness making a statement and how the memory can be misconstrued in this time frame. To follow this will be the issue of how much trust jurors-who have no legal training-put on to the eye witness testimony, which may be faltered. This paper references the works of primarily Wells and Olsen (2003) and Rodin (1987) and Schmechel et al. (2006) it will be argued that eye witness testimony is not always accurate, due to many features; inter racial identification, characteristics of the crime, response latency, and line up procedures therefore this paper will confirm that eyewitness testimonies should not be utilised in the criminal ju...
Eyewitness misidentification cost innocent people to end up in prison. Eyewitness misidentification is the single greatest cause of wrongful convictions in the United States, having played a role in more than 70% of original convictions later overturned by new DNA evidence(Dunn). This explains eyewitness misidentification is not a reliable solution to prison the suspect and deal with other solution. The suspect is effected because the suspect goes through terrible life for crime they did not commit and false witness hunts
Another factor associated with wrongful convictions is eyewitness misidentification. The Innocence Project identifies eyewitness misidentification as the single most important factor leading to wrongful convictions. Eyewitness misidentification is often an error due to witnesses being under high pressure, witnesses focusing on the weapon more than the offender, and police procedures when receiving an identification statement from a victim. A study
Eyewitness identification and testimony play a huge role in the criminal justice system today, but skepticism of eyewitnesses has been growing. Forensic evidence has been used to undermine the reliability of eyewitness testimony, and the leading cause of false convictions in the United States is due to misidentifications by eyewitnesses. The role of eyewitness testimony in producing false confessions and the factors that contribute to the unreliability of these eyewitness testimonies are sending innocent people to prison, and changes are being made in order to reform these faulty identification procedures.
The justice system depends on eyewitness evidence to convict offenders. Eyewitness is a difficult task to achieve in the justice system. According to Wise, Dauphinais, & Safer (2007), in 2002 one million offenders were convicted as felons in America. Out of those one million offenders, 5000 of them were innocent in 2002 (Dauphinais, 2007). The Ohio Criminal Justice survey states that 1 out of 200 felony criminal cases is a wrongful conviction (Dauphinais et al., 2007). According to Dauphinais et al., (2007), Dripps said that eyewitness error is a huge factor in cases of wrong convictions. A study conducted in 1987 indicated that in roughly 80,000 criminal cases, eyewitness error was the only sole evidence against the defendant
There has been considerable debate worldwide, regarding the accuracy of eyewitness testimony in the criminal justice system. Particularly, arguments have surrounded wrongful convictions that have resulted from incorrect eyewitness evidence (Areh, 2011; Howitt, 2012; Nelson, Laney, Bowman-Fowler, Knowles, Davis & Loftus, 2011). The purpose of this essay is to consider psychological research about the accuracy of eyewitness testimony and its placement in the criminal justice system. Firstly, this essay will define how eyewitnesses and their testimonies are used within the criminal justice system and the current debate surrounding its usage. Secondly, the impact of post-identification feedback will be used to show the affect on the confidence of a witness. Thirdly, studies around gender related differences will show how a witnesses gender can affect memory recall and accuracy. Fourthly, empirical studies will be used to highlight how a psychological experience called change blindness can cause mistakes in eyewitness identification. Finally, the effect of cross-examination will be used to explore the impact on eyewitness accuracy. It will be argued, that eyewitness testimony is not accurate and highly subjective, therefore, the criminal justice system must reduce the impact that eyewitness testimony is allowed to have. Developing better policies and procedures to avoid wrongful convictions by misled judges and jury members can do this.
In the court of law, eyewitnesses are expected to present evidence based upon information they acquired visually. However, due to memory processing, presenting this information accurately is not always possible. This paper will discuss the reliability of eyewitness testimony, its use in a relevant court case, and how the reasonable person standard relates to eyewitness testimony.
For example, the old man that lived beneath the boy and his father testified that he heard a fight between the boy and the father and heard the boy yell, “I’m gonna kill you,” along with a body hitting the ground, and then claims that he saw the boy running down the stairs. With this information, along with other powerful eyewitness testimonies, all but one of the jury members believed this boy was guilty. The power of eyewitness testimony is also shown in Loftus’s (1974) study. In this study, Loftus (1974) found that those who claimed to “see” something were usually believed even when their testimony is pointless. She discovered in her study that only 18 percent of people convicted if there was no eyewitness testimony, 72 percent of people convicted when someone declared, “That’s the one!”, and even when the witness only had 20/400 vision and was not wearing glasses and claimed “That’s the one!”, 68 percent of people still convicted the person. This proves that in 12 Angry Men and Loftus (1974) study, eyewitness testimony is very powerful and influential in one’s decision to convict a
Ricard, Suzanne, Thompson, Jennie. “Women’s Role in Serial Killing Teams: Reconstructing a Radical Feminist Perspective.” Critical Criminology 17(4): 261-275
Eyewitness testimony is especially vulnerable to error when the question is misleading or when there’s a difference in ethnicity. However, using an eyewitness as a source of evidence can be risky and is rarely 100% accurate. This can be proven by the theory of the possibility of false memory formation and the question of whether or not a memory can lie. For instance, a group of students saw the face of a young man with straight hair, then heard a description of the face supposedly written by another witness, one that wrongly mentioned light, curly hair. When they reconstructed the face using a kit of facial features, a third of their reconstructions contained the misleading detail, whereas only 5 percent contained it when curly hair was not mentioned (Page 359). This situation shows how misleading information from other sources can be profoundly altered.