Motherese in Language Aquisition

1197 Words3 Pages

Motherese in Language Aquisition 'Motherese' is known as 'Caretaker speech' or 'Parentese'; this is an important part of language acquisition. According to Sara Thorne's mastering advanced English language. Motherese means: the 'altered interaction of adults towards children'. Motherese is an important part of bringing up children. Because it gives children specific opportunities to take part in the discourse of speaking. In Motherese utterances are simplified; notation patterns are distinctive; extra information is given for clarification and questions invite direct participation. Adults will often expand on a child's speech and correct what they have said, For example: A child says "where my toys gone" and the parent corrects the child saying "where have my toys gone." Motherese differs quite markedly from speech between two adults. In Motherese these key features are apparent: *Vocabulary is simplified so that concrete objects are named in broad categories, for example: dog rather than spaniel or Labrador. Ball rather than football, cricket ball or tennis ball. 'Baby words' like doggie or moocow do not help raise, or bring up a child to learn academic English. *Conversations are generally based on concrete things that relate directly to the childs environment. *Sentence structures make use of pauses to stress the end of grammatical units.Certain sentence patterns occour regularly: Where is ----------?,do you want a------------?That's plainly a-----------. *Commands occur frequently and young children assimilate(take in information fully) and use them in their own speech. *Tag questions are questions a... ... middle of paper ... ...acitly assume that that children aren't worth speaking to, and don't have anything to say that is worth listening to. Such children learn to speak by overhearing streams of adult-to-adult speech (Heath, 1983). In some communities in New Guinea, mothers consciously try to teach their children language, but not in the style familiar to us, of talking to them indulgently. Rather, they wait until a third party is present, and coach the child as to the proper, adultlike sentences they should use (see Schieffelin & Eisenberg, 1981). Nonetheless, those children, like all children, grow up to be fluent language speakers. It surely must help children when their parents speak slowly, clearly, and succinctly to them, but their success at learning can't be explained by any special grammar-unveiling properties of parental babytalk.

Open Document