Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
False confessions research paper
True crimes false confessions
False confessions research paper
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
A false confession is an “admission plus a post admission narrative of a crime that the confessor did not commit” (Leo, 2009). Research shows that individuals falsely confess to crimes that they have not committed (Drizin & Leo, 2004; Kassin et al., 2010). Interrogations have been seen to lead to false confessions, which individuals are then incarcerated for crimes they did not commit. Some of these individuals are then later exonerated because of DNA evidence proving their innocence. Many of these cases are involving false confessions, which are given by individuals in coercive, police interrogations. “ In 15 to 20 percent of the DNA cases, police-induced false confessions were the primary cause of the wrongful conviction” (Leo, 2009). The …show more content…
problem here may be the techniques used that produce false confessions. History shows that confessions have often been extorted to save law enforcement officials the trouble and effort of obtaining valid and independent evidence (Haynes v. Washington). Safeguards exist to keep illegally obtained confessions out of the courtroom. The Miranda warnings exist to protect suspects from unknowingly incriminating themselves. Also, something exists called the “voluntariness requirement” which prevents coerced confessions from reaching the jury (Kassin et al., 2010). False confessions are difficult to discover because police do not typically keep records of them. It is difficult to prove the confessors innocence which makes them difficult to establish. (Kassin et al., 2010). Different types of false confessions exist.
Voluntary false confessions, compliant false confessions, persuaded false confessions. A voluntary false confession is one that is given in the absence of police interrogation (Kassin & Wrightsman, 1985). These may be the result of a psychological disturbance, desire for notoriety or fame, need to expiate guilt over imagined or real acts, desire to protect a criminal, provide an alibi for a different crime, or for revenge (Kassin & Wrightsman, 1985). Compliant false confessions are given in agreement to police coercion or pressure. These are typically to escape from the stress of an interrogation, to take advantage of a perceived suggestion or promise of leniency, or to avoid an anticipated harsh punishment (Ofshe & Leo, 1997). Persuaded false confessions are those which occur when “ police interrogation tactics cause an innocent suspect to doubt his memory and he becomes temporarily persuaded that it is more likely than not that he committed the crime, though he has no recollection of committing it (Leo, 2009). These types of false confessions cause suspects to suffer cognitive dissonance because of the accusers telling him that he committed a crime but the innocent suspect has no memory of it, so he believes that he just does not remember committing the crime. These types of false confessions occur much less than compliant false …show more content…
confessions. Three types of errors exist that can lead police interrogators into instigating false confessions. These are: misclassification error, coercion error, and contamination error. A misclassification error is an error on the police official’s part. It is when they believe that an innocent person is guilty. A coercion error is when “police use techniques that break down a suspects resistance to admitting guilt” (Hritz et al., 2010). Police make suspects feel as though there is no way out of the interrogation but to confess because of the evidence they have against them. Contamination error is when police make the suspects statements look persuasive. They will try and get the suspect to provide the answers that they are looking for by using leading questions. Police use verbal and nonverbal cues, and behavioral attitudes to detect deception among suspects (Kassin, 2010). This is shown to be not accurate because they are not more than 60% accurate at detecting truth. Regarding why the innocent confess to crimes they did not commit could be due to psychological distress, isolation, confrontation, and minimization. Isolation causes increased anxiety and a desire to escape. Minimization is when an interrogator is sympathetic about the crime and morally justifies it, making the suspect feel like the interrogator will be lenient if they simply confess. Confessions are the most incriminating piece of evidence against a defendant (Leo, 2009). To have a false confession would be just as incriminating against one who is innocent. Most civilians, jurors and criminal justice officials would assume that a detailed confession is true. Any other contradictory information or evidence against an innocent person who has confessed tends to be overridden by the confession (Leo & Ofshe, 1998). A confession sets the presumption of guilt among criminal justice officials, jurors, and the public eye. (Leo & Ofshe, 1998). Because of this, the confessed is treated harshly during the investigation and trial process. They are much more likely to be incarcerated before the trial, charged, pressured to plead guilty and then convicted (Leo, 2009). The existence of a confession creates confirmation and biases with officials and jurors causing them to have a negative outlook on the individual, causing them to interpret other information regarding their case in the worst way possible. For example, an eyewitness identification that would have been dismissed if there was no confession is treated as corroboration of the confessions truth. As this case moves through the criminal justice system it picks up more “collective force” and the fact that it was a false confession becomes seemingly more difficult to reverse. This set of events begins with the police after obtaining a confession. They will typically close the investigation, mark the case as solved and not look any farther to pursue any other leads, even if the confession is inconsistent or contradictory (Leo, 2009). Even when other evidence on the case comes out about the confession being false, police will still believe that the suspect is guilty and the confession is accurate. American police interrogators are not well trained about the risks of psychological interrogation and the idea of police-induced false confession (O’Danahue, 2004). They tend to assume that all confessions are true, and therefore they are assuming that everyone who confesses is guilty. This does not take into account the fact that a confession could be coerced, or could be a false confession. The fact that the criminal justice system tends to treat those who confess more harshly assumes that they do not take into account that an innocent person could have falsely confessed to a crime they never committed. There is some skepticism that exists with prosecutors about false confessions and they are reluctant to admit that a false confession occurred, even after there has been notable DNA evidence to establish the defendant’s innocence. Once a suspect has confessed, they are charged with the highest number of offenses, and also the highest types of offenses (Cassell & Hayman, 1996). Defense attorneys pressure suspects who confess more harshly and they pressure them to accept a plea to a lesser charge to avoid the higher sentence that will come from a jury conviction (Leo, 2009). “The confession operates as a kind of evidentiary bombshell which shatters the defense” (California V. Cahill, 1993). Judges tend to think that one who has confessed is automatically guilty and therefore they treat them like they deserve more punishment. Because judges are trained to not believe defendants claims of innocence, they rarely suppress confessions. If a case goes to trial, the jury treats a confession as a sign that the defendant is guilty rather than any other type of evidence. This happens especially in high profile cases where the confession gets a lot of publicity. In a study by Leo and Ofshe, they had 60 false confessions and they found that 73% of false confessors who went to trial were wrongfully convicted. This shows how dangerous false confessions can be in juries because they tend to blindly accept a confession as true, not taking into account any other evidence that a suspect may be innocent. Jurors fail to discount false confession evidence even when coercive methods were used to obtain the confession and other evidence exist to prove innocence (Leo, 2009). When a confession is retracted and a suspect pleads not guilty and goes to trial, a sequence of events is set into motion. A judge must determine whether or not the confession was voluntary and if it is admissible as evidence in a courtroom. After hearing the confession, the jury determines whether they believe that the defendant is guilty beyond a reasonable doubt. The ability that people have to distinguish whether or not a confession is true is not great, and secondly, most people believe that no one would confess to a crime that they did not commit. In a study by Kissin et. Al, they videotaped male prison inmates making true confessions to crimes they committed, and making up false confessions to crimes they did not commit. When investigators, and others viewed these tapes, the accuracy of distinguishing between the true confession and the false confession was not good. They ranged from 42% to 64% (Kassin et al., 2005). In another study by Kassin and Sukel, their results showed that confessions are very strong forms of evidence and people do not discount confessions even when they are coerced. Mock jurors were presented with either a low, high pressure confession or no confession for their controls. Those in the high- pressure control group believed that the confession was involuntary and did not influence their decision. This was the same control group that had a high conviction rate, even when participants were asked to disregard confessions that they believed were coerced. Confessions are “inherently prejudicial and highly damaging to a defendant, even if it is the product of coercive interrogation, even if it is supported by no other evidence, and even if it is ultimately proven false beyond any reasonable doubt” (Drizin & Leo, 2004, p. 959). The Miranda Rights exist to protect suspects from situations that could produce false confessions in the U.S.
this came up through the Supreme Court Decision in Miranda v. Arizona. There was concern with the powers of the state through law enforcement overbearing “the will of citizen suspects, thus threatening their Constitutional right to avoid self-incrimination” (Kissin et. Al., 2009). The court required police officers to inform suspects of their right to remain silent and the right to legal counsel before confessions. This requirement exists to separate the threatening power that the police have relative to the suspect, hopefully reducing the likeliness of coercive confessions. In cases that involved challenges to the validity of the waiver of rights, courts were required to apply a test regarding the admissibility of the confession at trial (Kassin et al., 2009). If a waiver to the rights to silence and counsel was not made voluntarily, knowingly, and intelligently, then statements made by a defendant would be inadmissible. After the Miranda rights decision came out, Gault made these rights apply to youth who faced delinquency charges in juvenile court. Though these rights exist, no research evidence exists that they really achieved their goal. Officers do give suspects a warning before they take statements, but research does not show that confessions are more or less reliable before the Miranda rights existed. Paul Cassell, had believed that the confession and
conviction rates dropped greatly as a result of the warning and waiver requirements, and thus triggering the release of dangerous criminals (Cassell & Hayman, 1996). Others believe that this analysis was incorrect and that the declines were insubstantial if they existed (Kassin et al., 2009). Police must provide suspects a description of their rights that is understandable, and to be valid, the suspect must understand these rights. The Miranda rights clearly describe the rights which suspects must be informed of. Also, the decision includes an appendix where an example of the rights exists, but it is up to each police department to come up with a version of the rights. A study by Rogers et al. examines 560 Miranda warning forms that are used by the police in the United States. Content, format, and wording were looked at in regards to the different form used. They found that these different forms of Miranda warnings used conveyed in their messages. 32% told suspects that they could obtain legal counsel without charge. Whether or not suspects are truly aware of their rights is in question because of this. There are certain individuals who are more likely to be coerced into making false confessions. Certain people are better able to handle the pressure, and others crack under the pressure in interrogations. “Those who are highly suggestible or compliant are more likely to confess falsely” (Leo, 2009). Those who are highly suggestible have worse memories, have low self- esteem, low levels of assertiveness, high anxiety levels, and have personality factors that make them more vulnerable to the pressure of interrogation and therefore make them more likely to deal with false confessions (Leo, 2009). Those who are very compliant have a conflict avoidant personality, and are eager to please others, especially those in authority (Gudjonsson, 2003). Those who have developmental disabilities or cognitive impairments also suffer from vulnerability to false confessions. Also in that group are juveniles and those who are mentally ill. Developmentally disabled individuals do not have the average cognitive and intellectual functioning. They have low intelligence and are generally not able to concentrate well, nor do they have good memory. They suffer from poor communication and conceptual skills. They have trouble understanding things and do not understand consequences well. This makes them very easy to manipulate by other individuals. It would also be very easy for a police officer to get them to repeat false statements or agree with them that could be incriminating, even though it is not the truth. The developmentally disabled can be compared to child. They have similar cognitive levels. It is very easy to get a child to agree to what you are saying because they are easily influenced. They also get confused by statements which they do not understand. It is Important that false confessions are taken into account in the criminal justice system. Research shows how and why innocent people confess to serious crimes that they never committed. Reforms should be put in place to make sure suspects actually understand their Miranda rights when officials make them aware. Also, criminal justice professionals should take precautions towards the use of false confessions in court, and jury’s should be instructed on them. Police should have limits during interrogations, and individual’s lives should be held at a higher standard than winning a case. False confessions could incriminate an innocent person to life in prison. DNA exonerations do not happen to every innocent person, thus professionals must do everything in their power to keep the rate of innocent people incriminated as low as possible.
The Supreme Court ruled that due to the coercive nature of the custodial interrogation by police, no confession could be admissible under the Fifth Amendment self-incrimination Clause and Sixth Amendment right to an attorney unless a suspect has been made aware to his rights and the suspect had then waived them
Defenders of the Miranda decision say that fewer crimes solved are for a good reason. They believe that law enforcement officers were forced to stop coercive questioning techniques that are unconstitutional. Over the years, the Supreme Court has watered down its stance in saying that the Miranda rules are not constitutional obligations, but rather “prophylactic” safeguards intended to insure that officers do not force a confession from a suspect. The need for both effective law enforcement as well as protection of society dictates the need for potential alternatives to the limitations of Miranda that would simultaneously protect the interest of society in effective law enforcement while at the same time providing protection to suspects against unconstitutional force (www.ncpa.org).
Miranda Vs Arizona was a United States Supreme Court case in 1966. The court “ruled that a criminal suspect must make a knowing, intelligent, and voluntary decision to waive certain constitutional rights prior to questioning” (Ortmeier, 2005, 285). This ruling meant that suspects must be aware of their right to remain silent and that if they choose to speak to the police the conversation can be used against them in a court of law. If they do decide to speak under police it must not be under false promises
McCann, Joseph. “A Conceptual Framework for Identifying Various Types of Confessions.” Behavioral Sciences and the Law 16 (1998): 441-453. Web. 8 January 2014.
This is derived from the rights Americans have to not be forced to testify against themselves in a criminal case. But, the Fifth Amendment also protects against double jeopardy and gives people charged with a felony the right to a grand jury indictment (Bohm & Haley, 2011). Double jeopardy basically states that if a conviction or acquittal was reached in a criminal case, the person can no longer be tried again for the same offense (Bohm & Haley, 2011). The procedural rights for self-incrimination are also applied to any custodial situations the police conduct. To ensure that statements, or confessions a suspect makes are allowed in court there is a two-prong tests that should be followed. First, is the person considered to be in a custodial situation and two, are the police intending to ask incriminating questions. If yes is the answers to both then the suspect must be read his or her rights. This is known as giving someone his or her Miranda rights derived from the famous case
The Central Park Jogger case is one of false confessions to a crime, with a little help from police, which the defendants did not commit. Evidence taken at the crime scene did exclude the defendants, however, because of videotaped confessions they were sentenced to prison for a crime they admitted to committing even though they did not. It was not until many years later did the original perpetrator step forward from prison to admit he was the one who committed the crime with evidence (DNA) and firsthand knowledge of the scene. The five original defendants were released from prison but until serving a lengthy term. There are cues that can be noticed when investigators are conducting preliminary interviews that have a very high rate of success in determining the guilt or innocence of an individual. Some of these cues may be verbal such as a rehearsed response (Kassin, 2005). Other types of cues may be nonverbal body language such as a slouching (Kassin, 2005).
There are nine steps to the interrogation process, but before the steps are implemented, there’s an initial interview to determine guilt or innocence. During this time, the interrogator attempts to create a rapport with the suspect by using casual conversation to establish a non-threating atmosphere. Often time, people are more comfortable when they feel they can relate to the person they are talking to, so the interrogator may claim to share some common beliefs or interest. If the suspect starts talking to the interrogator about harmless things, it becomes harder to stop talking or start lying later, after when the discussion turns to crime (dying words). In the initial investigation, the investigator will observe the suspects verbal and non-verbal reactions, this information will help establish a baseline reaction before the stress commences; later on in the investigation, the baseline will help the interrogator determine if the suspect is telling the truth or lying. Now the investigation can proceed with the nine-step process. First step, direct positive confrontation, involves directly confronting the suspect with a statement that it is known that he or she committed the crime. Often, the police lie and describe nonexistent evidence that points to the suspect as the offender. The second step, theme development, is the step in which the police present a hypothesis about the
The act of interrogation has been around for thousands of years. From the Punic Wars to the war in Iraq, interrogating criminals, prisoners or military officers in order to receive advantageous information has been regularly used. These interrogation techniques can range from physical pain to emotional distress. Hitting an individual with a whip while they hang from a ceiling or excessively questioning them may seem like an ideal way to get them to reveal something, but in reality it is ineffective and . This is because even the most enduring individual can be made to admit anything under excruciating circumstances. In the Fifth Amendment of the Bill of Rights there is a provision (“no person shall be compelled in any criminal case to be a witness against himself” ) which reflects a time-honored common principle that no person is bound to betray him or herself or can be forced to give incriminating evidence. This ideology of self-incrimination has been challenged heavily over the past s...
...igations today has a huge impact on false confessions. The Reid Technique is being criticized in the media because of its guilt-presumptive, aggressive, and psychologically manipulative nature. It is based on a series of assumptions that lack scientific support, and by using it they are creating hostile and coercive environment for the interrogation. The fact that they try to pass these confessions off as voluntary should also be an issue against using them since we know they are usually coerced. There are two alternatives to the Reid technique being used to interview suspects. These do not use coercion and manipulation to get confessions. The first is the PEACE Model, which is an interview technique that is more ethical, and the other technique is Cognitive interviewing which is used by police as a memory technique used to enhance the retrieval of their memory.
Mental retardation or suspects with low intelligence quotients (IQ) are easily manipulated by police comments and interrogation tactics. Those suspects usually do not understand the law or the consequences of a confession. They may want to please the police officer by being accommodating or agreeable. They may just wa...
Garrett, B. L. (n.d.). The Substance of False Confessions. Criminal Justice Collection. Retrieved November 23, 2010, from find.galegroup.com.uproxy.library.dc-uoit.ca/gtx/retrieve.do?contentSet=IAC-Documents&resultListType=RESULT_LIST&qrySerId=Locale%28en%2C%2C%29%3AFQE%3D%28su%2CNone%2C28%29%22Wrongful+Convictions+%28Law%29%22%3AAnd%3ALQE%3D%28RE%2CNone%2C3%29ref%24&sgHitCo
Miranda v. Arizona is a very important activist decision that required police to inform criminal suspects of their rights before they could be interrogated. These rights include: the right to remain silent, that anything you say can and will be used against you in a court of law, you have a right to an attorney, if you cannot afford an attorney one will be appointed to you be the court. In this case the Fifth Amendment's right that a person may not be forced to incriminate one's self was interpreted in an activist way as meaning that one must be aware of this right before on is interrogated by the police. Prior to this ruling it was common practice to force and coerce confessions from criminal suspects who did not know they had the right not to incriminate themselves.
In both cases, confirmation bias and investigative bias played a considerable role in all eight false confessions; as well as psychological coercion and the abuse of power by investigators when interrogating the suspects. Many situational and individual factors came into play during the case such as the amount of time all eight were interrogated, intrinsic and extrinsic evidence/ maximization used against them. For example, both Joe Dick and Carole Richardson were the most susceptible to psychological manipulation. Joe because of possible cognitive reasoning’s that I personally feel weren’t mentioned, but was implied; Richardson because of substance abuse during the period in which she was investigated.
Miranda also protects suspects from overzealous police officers. Although most law-enforcement agents in the United States are decent men and women, some abuse their power. They may try to coerce suspects into giving false confessions. Time and time again, we read of cases where suspects were forced to make confessions because an overzealous or prejudiced police officers want to close a case. The story of Rubin Hurricane Carter, made popular by the motion picture of the same name, demonstrated how lives could be destroyed when vindictive and manipulating detectives abuse their power. The Miranda Warning helps keep abuses in check. If the law is used correctly, the guilty would receive their due punishment. When police officers inform suspects of their rights before interrogation, it is very unlikely that the judge presiding over any case would throw out statements made during questioning.
Leo, R and Ofshe R. The Social Psychology of Police Interrogation: The Theory and Classification of True and False Confessions. 16 Studies in Law, Politics and Society 189,