Wait a second!
More handpicked essays just for you.
More handpicked essays just for you.
Opposing thoughts of the causes of altruism
Opposing thoughts of the causes of altruism
Essay on the order of human altruism
Don’t take our word for it - see why 10 million students trust us with their essay needs.
Question 1:
Altruism is a behavior that benefits another individual despite the cost to oneself (Sparrow and Spaniol, 2018). Consequently, altruism in humans may have evolved as a result of either kin selection, group/multilevel selection or reciprocal altruism. By definition, kin selection involves the tendency to assist those of close relation compared to those more distantly related to oneself (Chaung and Wu, 2017). Secondly, multilevel selection theory involves group led benefits where the fitness of the group outweighs the costs to the individual (Ogorman et al., 2008). Thirdly, reciprocal altruism comprises cooperation between individuals where one helps another with the expectation that the other individual will help them out in return at a later date (Takano et al., 2016). Of the three types of altruism, I hypothesize that kin selection best explains and fits with the evolution of altruism in humans.
The kin selection hypothesis, also called the inclusive fitness theory, predicts altruistic tendencies will be higher towards another individual when the benefit to the individual and relatedness are greater than the cost to the actor (Foster et al., 2005).
…show more content…
Inclusive fitness theory supports the evolution of altruism in species, stating individuals will work towards increasing their fitness through the increased production of offspring or by aiding in the reproduction of relatives who hold similar genes to oneself (Chaung and Wu, 2017). The thought is if one were to partake in altruistic behaviors that benefit another individual, such as a close relative one would increase the fitness of that individual and, thus, increase the number of offspring being created that are of close relation to the altruistic individual. Furthermore, inclusive fitness theory presumes that altruism evolved to increase the percentage of the actor’s genes in a population, with research indicating that these mechanisms of altruistic behavior in humans follow with inclusive theory in the relatedness of the individual as well as being based on emotional closeness to an individual (Korchmaros and Kenny, 2001). However, when looking at it from the perspective of emotional closeness a major limitation in the theory of kin selection can be seen. The one limitation of the kin altruism theory is the presence of emotional closeness within a population, where individuals will tend to help others who they find emotionally close to themselves, such as friends than non-emotionally close kin (Korchmaros and Kenny, 2001). Looking further into the evolutionary mechanisms responsible for kin altruism in humans, parental investment should be investigated, especially since male parenting in mammals are found in less than 5% of species, however with humans, this number is greatly increased (Geary, 2015). The principle of parental care involves the ratio of the cost to the individual and improved offspring survivorship. The concept of parental investment has indirect and direct benefits on the offspring, where offspring survival is increased directly through resource acquisition from the parents, and the indirect benefit of increasing the ability of the individual to pass on its genetic material (Geary, 2015). This principle closely follows the principle of kin selection where they both work towards increasing genetic inheritance through the population. Additionally, Parental investment has the potential to expand to grandparents, as shown by kin altruism, whereby the grandparents will depict parental care towards grandchildren due to the genetic relationship between the two (Jeon and Buss, 2007). Jeon and Buss theorize that kin altruism should follow through to cousins as well as grandchildren who hold a direct line of relation. For this study, Jeon and Buss (2007) created a mathematical model to determine which cousins an individual would be more likely to show kin altruistic tendencies towards based on relatedness and compared these computer-generated numbers to the observed findings, showing that individuals will have cousin-specific behaviors towards those they sense more genetic relation too. A limitation of this study would detail the inability to without a doubt distinguish an individual's ability to recognize the variability in the genetic relation of non-identified cousins in the family. An improvement on this study to distinguish emotional relatedness to actual genetic relatedness would be to use individuals who are not already aware of the family ties between cousins to show the degree of altruism towards these individuals. Overall, kin selection best explains the evolution of altruism in humans with the factors of parental investment, relatedness, emotional closeness towards family members, and increasing fitness through the aiding of relatives who hold similar genes to oneself. Furthermore, kin selection follows with the concept in altruism, where a behavior benefits another individual despite the costs to oneself, however, with kin selection the altruistic behavior ultimately benefits the fitness of the individual from purely a stance of increasing one's genes in the population. As such, I think kin selection best explains and fits with the evolution of altruism in humans. Question 2: A phylogenetic tree based on a single gene will not accurately represent the evolutionary history of species due to both lateral gene transfer, and the lack of DNA fragments to determine gene changes in long-extinct species resulting in a tree of life that will reflect only known species. Accordingly, lateral gene transfer refers to the transfer of genes between organisms and contributes to around 20-30% of the variation in genes between species (Bapteste et al., 2004). This poses an issue with phylogenetic tree reconstruction due to the sharing of genes between related and unrelated species causing certain species to appear more closely related when looking at a single gene. In contrast, the inability to use genes from extinct species causes large holes in the phylogenetic tree being constructed from known genes. This is because genes cannot be taken from ancient fossils or that only phenotypes can be inferred from a fossil, while genotypes are indeterminable in most fossil types. Firstly, lateral gene transfer is the transfer of genes between organisms and can occur on account of viruses, transposons or from natural processes in most bacteria and archaea (Khan et al., 2016).
These naturally occurring processes allow for lateral gene transfer between prokaryotes. For instances, prokaryotes are capable of taking up DNA from their environment in a process called transformation, transfer genes through transduction with phage particles, and many other gene transfer processes (Ku and Martin, 2016). The transfer of genes may increase the mutation rate in the genomes of these organisms with the insertion of new DNA segments into their own genome with deleterious, beneficial or silent effects with the potential to increase the rate of change in organisms and thus the rate of
evolution. The mechanisms of lateral gene transfer have contributed to the spread of antibiotic-resistant genes, with antibiotics installing strong selective pressures for resistance and the uptake of naked DNA elements in the environment contributing to the acquirement of antibiotic resistance genes through lateral gene transfer (Sieber et al., 2017). Furthermore, Sieber and associates (2017) identified a lateral gene transfer occurrence between humans and bacteria with a 685 base pair fragment of human DNA found. This highlights the issue of constructing a tree of life from a single gene as, if one were to use the single gene found in humans and certain bacteria, the tree of life would have the potential to depict humans as more closely related to a species of bacteria than that bacteria is to other bacteria. A further example showing the effects of lateral gene transfer on the construction of a tree using a single gene is seen in a study by Kanhere and Vingron (2009) who found that out of 171 different gene transfer incidences between Archaea and Bacteria, 118 of them were responsible for metabolic genes and 53 of them involved translation genes occurring between bacteria phyla resulting in dramatically different phylogenetic trees when these genes were compared to other genes. Due to the prevalence of lateral gene transfer, it is difficult to determine which genes are a result of the transfer and which represent gene losses and gains in a phylogenetic tree. Secondly, the inability to use genes from long-extinct species causes large holes in the phylogenetic tree and difficulty in its construction from known genes. According to Moret and Warnow (2002), phylogenetic trees are constructed using DNA, RNA or amino acids for genes, yet a key challenge in constructing a tree from genes is inferring relation from ancient species where DNA fragments are not available. Much of the tree of life is comprised of species that are no longer around with multiple mass extinction events in our history. This causes large gaps and the tree to represent only extant organism. The fossils of extinct species may be made through the replacement of organic material with new material to create casts or per-mineralized bones (Freeman and Herron, 2001). Due to the organic material being replaced by minerals in most fossils, there are no traces of DNA to compare genes to extant species. However, some fossils are of remains suspended in amber or preserved in ice. The remains may contain trace amounts of DNA, unfortunately, the half-life of DNA does not allow for the preservation of ancient genes. For instance when extracting DNA from ancient papyri, researcher found the half-life of DNA to be between 19 and 24 years, resulting in the total loss of any DNA fragments in the papyri sheets made from ancient papyrus trees to be within 532 and 672 years (Marota et al., 2002). The half-life of DNA in this organic material does not leave much hope for the extraction of preserved DNA fragments from fossils. Nevertheless, in another study on DNA half-life, it was determined that small fragments of DNA can survive at temperatures below 5 degrees Celsius for 800-450 thousand years (Allentroft et al., 2012). Despite this increase in the lifespan of a fragment of DNA when subjected to extremely cold temperatures, the lack of DNA in remains older than 450,000 years leaves significant gaps in the evolutionary tree when using genes for comparison. Ultimately in regard to the lack of DNA fragments and the abundance of lateral gene transfers, a phylogenetic tree made from a single known gene does not accurately represent the evolutionary history of species. The lack of DNA fragments from ancient species and thus the inability to determine the genes responsible for many species in our history generates far too many unknown variables and gaps in the evolutionary tree for an accurate representation. Additionally, the myriad of instances of lateral gene transfer between both closely and distantly related species causes discrepancies in the branching of species on the phylogenetic tree when comparing a single gene. In conclusion, a single gene should not be relied on to create the tree of life.
Altruism is selfless acts like someone willingly sacrificing their life for their child’s. When people show acts of altruism it is usually because they feel empathy for the person. They have feelings that reflect on how that person is feeling in the situation. We sacrifice ourselves for strangers in need to help reduce our personal distress of seeing them in need. Another reason is experiencing the feelings of the person in need. If a person sees someone having car trouble they will want to help because they remember having car trouble with no one around to help. They are sacrificing their wellbeing by pulling over to help, they could be putting themselves in a bad situation if the person is a criminal.
Khaled Hosseini’s The Kite Runner provides incredibly valuable insight into the intertwining concepts of identity and personal happiness, as does A Complicated Kindness by Miriam Toews. Undeniably, a healthy cognisance of personal identity is crucial to mental tranquility and happiness. Insecurity, for example is unilaterally viewed as an unsettling character flaw precisely because of the inherent implication of untrue personal identity. Accurately understanding one’s true personal identity is the backbone of happiness, as seen in The Kite Runner and A Complicated Kindness. Furthermore, it can be said that personal flaws, strengths and collectivist mentalities surrounding both Amir and Nomi’s characters ultimately contribute to their respective happiness or lack thereof, at the end of both stories.
Human social affinity appears to be much more interesting than “nice people are liked” and “aggressive people are disliked.” A more nuanced view emerges when considering the evolutionary functions of behaviors as they relate to dominance. The bulk of psychology considers antisociality and prosociality as opposite ends of a single continuum and, as such, assumes that they serve opposing functions. Here, I have considered them as serving the same function as two sides of the same coin (437).
According to Merriam-Webster dictionary, altruism is defined as an “unselfish regard for or devotion to the welfare of others”. In simpler terms, altruism is unconditional kindness. Altruistic behavior has been a controversial and obscure subject among biologists, including Charles Darwin, who was one of the first to try and explain this phenomenon. “To explain the evolution of altruism, Charles Darwin suggested that natural selection could act on groups as well as individuals--an idea known as group selection”(Schwartz). Group selection, which was based off of Darwin’s theory of natural selection, was the theory that an individual organism would sacrifice itself (seemingly selflessly) for the good of the entire population, hoping that its actions would save the group from annihilation and therefore allow for the continuation of the group’s gene flow into its descendants. Darwin explained that groups with members “ready to give aid to each other and sacrifice themselves for the com...
The two competing theoretical frameworks that attempt to explain the development of morality are cognitive-behavioral and cognitive-developmental. The cognitive-behavioral approach is taken by Liebert, and the cognitive-developmental approach is taken by Kohlberg.
Altruism regards the individual life as something one may be required to sacrifice for the sake of
behavior for the benefit of other." (Recent Work on Human Altruism and Evolution) Another popularized theory by Richard Dawkins states that an individual who behaves more altruistic towards others who share its genes will tend to reproduce those genes. But since we always want something in exchange for something we did, can this truly exist, or is it just an illusion? On Jan.
For someone who believes in psychological egoism, i t is difficult to find an action that would be acknowledged as purely altruistic. In practice, altruism, is the performance of duties to others with no view to any sort of personal...
According to the article, Altruism and helping behavior, it is common for people to help others. Altruism is defined as “the desire to help another person even if it doesn’t benefit the helper” (Altruism and Helping Behavior. Print.). Helping behavior is “any act that is intended to benefit another person”
On the other hand, cells that have resistance from the start or acquire it later may survive. At the same time, when antibiotics attack disease-causing bacteria, they also attack benign bacteria. This process eliminates drug-susceptible bacteria and favors bacteria that are resistant. Two things happen, populations of non-resistant and harmless bacteria are diminished, and because of the reduction of competition from these harmless and/or susceptible bacteria, resistant forms of disease-causing bacteria proliferate. As the resistant forms of the bacteria proliferate, there is more opportunity for genetic or chromosomal mutation (spontaneous DNA mutation (1)) or transformation, that comes about either through a form of microbial sex (1) or through the transference of plasmids, small circles of DNA (1), which allow bacteria to interchange genes with ease.
In Barry H. Corey’s book, Love Kindness, the underestimated, devalued virtue of kindness is explored. Kindness is a powerful, gentle strength that is often underestimated and devalued. Living a life of kindness is not for the meek or the prideful for this is a humbling, bold, and whimsical way of being. Love and kindness are difficult to separate because kindness is a result of love in its most authentic state. Love and kindness are the keys to moments of vulnerability. In that moment of authentic love and kindness barriers are demolished and people are reached in their brokenness.
Gintis, Herbert, Samuel Bowles, Robert Boyd, and Ermst Fehr. “Explaining Altruistic Behavior in Humans.” Evolution and Human Behavior 24 (2003): 153-172. Web. 5 Feb. 2012
One form is generalized reciprocity, which is the giving of goods without expectation of a return of equal value at any definite future time. Generalized reciprocity occurs mainly between individuals who are emotionally attached to one another and have a responsibility to help one another on the basis of need. In the United States, parents who provide their children with shelter, food, vehicles, college educations, and interest-free loans are practicing generalized reciprocity. Giving without the expectation of a quick and equivalent return should also occur between certain other kinds of social relations, such as wives and husbands, siblings, and sometimes close friends. Among certain groups of people more goods are exchanged using this form than any other. For example, most members of small hunting and gathering groups are expected to share food and be generous with their possessions. Generalized reciprocity happens in all human populations and is the dominant mode of exchange in very small groups in which all or most members are relatives.
There are many different factors that influence a child’s behaviour. It is up to early childhood educators to know these factors and how to help them overcome the challenges. Firstly, the environment and the people within in it, have an important role in influencing the behaviour of children. When teachers create a calm and secure environment, they are indirectly influencing the children’s behaviour. Gordon and Browne (2014), state that children are least likely to misbehave when provided with toys and activities that are entertaining and engaging. Therefore, children are able to feel relaxed and are more willing to listen to adults. Having an environment with enough space that allows children to move from different areas, without causing
Autism is an abnormal behavior in childhood that cause the child to have poor social skills, socially inappropriate repetitive behaviors, scattered speech or to be unable to verablly communicate with others. Altruism is caused by genetic factors as well as environmentally. Autism can range from mild to severe, with the milder form only making things such as altruism and logic difficult. Where as with the most severe a child will need help being fed, changed, etc. In 2015 researchers found that 1 in every 5 children was effected with autism as compared to 1 and 86 in 2007. Though there is no known cause of what can possibly be causing altuism, a lot of people point to measel shots as a cause. Another childhood disorder that is very common among